Home Email this page Print this page Bookmark this page Decrease font size Default font size Increase font size
Noise & Health  
 CURRENT ISSUE    PAST ISSUES    AHEAD OF PRINT    SEARCH   GET E-ALERTS    
 
 
Similar in PUBMED
   Search Pubmed for
   Search in Google Scholar for
 Related articles
Email Alert *
Add to My List *
* Registration required (free)  
 


 
   Abstract
  Introduction
  Methods
  Results
  Discussion
  Conclusions
  Acknowledgments
   References
   Article Figures
   Article Tables
 

 Article Access Statistics
    Viewed12552    
    Printed612    
    Emailed16    
    PDF Downloaded35    
    Comments [Add]    
    Cited by others 7    

Recommend this journal

 


 
  Table of Contents    
ARTICLE  
Year : 2012  |  Volume : 14  |  Issue : 57  |  Page : 47-51
Music to whose ears? The effect of social norms on young people's risk perceptions of hearing damage resulting from their music listening behavior

1 National Acoustic Laboratories, Chatswood; The Hearing CRC, Melbourne, Australia
2 National Acoustic Laboratories, Chatswood, Australia

Click here for correspondence address and email
Date of Web Publication18-Apr-2012
 
  Abstract 

Professional and community concerns about the potentially dangerous noise levels for common leisure activities has led to increased interest on providing hearing health information to participants. However, noise reduction programmes aimed at leisure activities (such as music listening) face a unique difficulty. The noise source that is earmarked for reduction by hearing health professionals is often the same one that is viewed as pleasurable by participants. Furthermore, these activities often exist within a social setting, with additional peer influences that may influence behavior. The current study aimed to gain a better understanding of social-based factors that may influence an individual's motivation to engage in positive hearing health behaviors. Four hundred and eighty-four participants completed questionnaires examining their perceptions of the hearing risk associated with listening to music listening and asking for estimates of their own and their peer's music listening behaviors. Participants were generally aware of the potential risk posed by listening to personal stereo players (PSPs) and the volumes likely to be most dangerous. Approximately one in five participants reported using listening volumes at levels perceived to be dangerous, an incidence rate in keeping with other studies measuring actual PSP use. However, participants showed less awareness of peers' behavior, consistently overestimating the volumes at which they believed their friends listened. Misperceptions of social norms relating to listening behavior may decrease individuals' perceptions of susceptibility to hearing damage. The consequences of hearing health promotion are discussed, along with suggestions relating to the development of new programs.

Keywords: Adolescents, hearing, music listening, noise-induced hearing loss, social norms

How to cite this article:
Gilliver M, Carter L, Macoun D, Rosen J, Williams W. Music to whose ears? The effect of social norms on young people's risk perceptions of hearing damage resulting from their music listening behavior. Noise Health 2012;14:47-51

How to cite this URL:
Gilliver M, Carter L, Macoun D, Rosen J, Williams W. Music to whose ears? The effect of social norms on young people's risk perceptions of hearing damage resulting from their music listening behavior. Noise Health [serial online] 2012 [cited 2023 Dec 7];14:47-51. Available from: https://www.noiseandhealth.org/text.asp?2012/14/57/47/95131

  Introduction Top


There is increasing concern that many everyday leisure activities have the potential to expose individuals to unsafe noise levels. Attendance at concerts, clubs, live bands and listening to personal stereos have all been identified as potentially damaging to hearing, and have become the focus for prevention messages. Voluntary exposure to such noise sources however carries inherent difficulties for hearing health promotion. If the leisure activity includes noise as a sought-after objectives (e.g., music listening) rather than a mere by-product (e.g., recreational shooting), noise exposure reduction is not just viewed as a low priority but as something that many may perceive as actually incompatible with leisure enjoyment. Reducing noise exposure in leisure activities, therefore, often relies on the choices made by individuals in relation to their own noise exposure behaviors. This behavior is in turn motivated by individuals' attitudes toward noise reduction exposure and noise reduction.

The use of personal stereo players (PSPs; e.g., MP3 players, iPods, etc.) provides a relevant example. When using a PSP, it is up to the individual to decide how loud they set the volume and the length of time they will spend listening. It should be noted that external measures such as volume-limiting software on PSPs can only have limited success in reducing the risk of noise-induced hearing loss (NIHL), as they generally do not limit the duration at which an individual uses the device. The maximum output from PSPs has been reported to be from 96 to 107 dB Keith, Michaud, and Chiu; [1] Portnuff and Fligor; [2] Williams and Purnell, [3] depending on the make/model and the accessories (e.g. speakers, earbuds, headphones) used. At a volume of 101 dB, noise exposure guidelines recommend a maximum of 15 mins of exposure, a duration easily exceeded by listening only to four to five modern music tracks. Consequently, PSP use has come under serious scrutiny in relation to its potential to result in hearing damage. As the actual volume and exact duration levels will vary based on individual preferences, quantifying the threat posed by PSPs can be difficult, and estimates of those at risk vary. Although few individuals would listen at the maximum levels reported above, a study by Vogel et al. [4] found that nearly half of the adolescents surveyed listened at volumes higher than 75% of the available player range. Furthermore, one-third of the PSP users also reported experiencing symptoms of hearing damage, including needing to increase the volume over time Vogel et al. [4] These figures are supported by measurements of actual listening levels and self-report durations, which suggest that 17-25% of PSP users may be listening to PSPs at potentially harmful levels Williams. [5],[6] Although only a minority of PSP users may actually be at risk for hearing damage, when the increasingly widespread popularity of PSPs are considered, this can equate to a relatively large population who are at risk for NIHL Fligor. [7]

Promoting healthy listening habits to PSP users relies on motivating the individual to not only monitor their own behavior but to also modify it as required. Research surveys have found that young people generally appear to be aware of the theoretical risk posed by high-level listening. However, few report taking or planning to take personal action to reduce their exposure Australian Hearing, [8] with a suggestion that many may not perceive themselves to be personally at risk, and thus find it unnecessary to change their behavior. Further information is therefore required regarding young people's personal beliefs about their susceptibility to hearing damage from leisure activities, in particular their attitudes regarding the risk to their hearing from PSPs.

Leisure activities are strongly linked to social behavior. As a result, attitudes about associated risk are accordingly influenced by social-based factors, including peer influences. Social factors are well recognised generally as influencing health behaviors, and are particularly important in relation to behavior in young adults and adolescents. Specifically, perceptions of social norms (i.e., judgements about what is "normal" behavior within the social group) can be used as a guideline of what is acceptable or approved, and can work to directly influence an individual's behavior (e.g., Theory of planned behaviour; Ajzen. [9],[10] That is, individuals judge behavior against the social norm of their peers and/or modify their own behavior to match that which is perceived to be "normal" or even further, as desirable (or, "cool"). Even when the reality of target behavior is difficult to accurately observe, individuals still carry beliefs about how they think others behave, social norm beliefs that may or may not match with reality. An examination of how individuals perceive peers' behavior may therefore provide better insight into individuals' understanding of their own behavior.

To date, social factors such as social norms have generally received relatively little attention in hearing health research. This study aimed to investigate perceived social norms in relation to individuals' perceptions of risk relating to their PSP listening behavior. Participants' perception of risk, their perceptions of their own behaviour and that of their peers was examined.


  Methods Top


Participants

The data presented was collected from 486 participants recruited for a large-scale study examining audiological data along with knowledge of and attitudes toward hearing and noise exposure. iHEAR; Carter. [11] Participants were mainly recruited through high schools and higher educational colleges/universities in rural and metropolitan areas of NSW, Australia.

Participants were classified into age categories based on their current educational status: Early High School (grades 7-9), Senior High School (grades 10-12) and Young Adulthood (18+ no longer at school). There were equivalent numbers of males and females in each group ([Table 1], along with age breakdown).
Table 1: Participants by age and gender

Click here to view


Ethics

The project received ethics approval from the Australian Hearing Human Research Ethics Committee and approval was given through the New South Wales, Department of Education and Training to conduct testing within schools. All participants were required to provide informed consent prior to participation in the project. Parents/guardians of participants under the age of 18 years were additionally required to provide written consent for their child to participate in the project.

Materials

The overall study involved collection of audiological data, demographic information and a survey of participant' knowledge of and attitudes toward hearing and noise exposure. iHEAR; Carter. [11] Results for the current study are drawn from a select sample of items within the large-scale questionnaires used as part of the study's testing protocol. To minimise potential confound by parents/peers, these items were presented by the audiologist as a semi-structured interview with the participant on the day of testing.

The questionnaire was developed with the aim of gaining a better understanding of the knowledge and attitudes of young people regarding hearing and noise. Questions related to the participants' beliefs about the risk posed to hearing by music listening in general, estimates of their own listening behavior and their peers' listening behaviors. A number of questions focussed on personal music listening behaviors (e.g., using PSPs and car/home stereos) as this was identified as an activity common to the majority of the participants (regardless of age).

The focus of this report is on participants' responses to five specific questions relating to perceptions of their own, and their peers', listening behaviors. Participants were asked if they believed using a PSP could be a risk to people's hearing (No; Maybe/Yes). Those answering maybe/yes to this item were then asked to use a 10-point scale [Figure 1] to estimate the volume at which they believed music volumes may pose a risk to hearing (Q1b).
Figure 1: Volume response scale

Click here to view


If participants reported using PSPs, they were asked to use this scale to indicate the volume they generally listened to music (Q2, Q3) and the volume at which they believed their friends generally listened to music (Q4, Q5). Responses on the 10-point scale were converted to equivalent percentages for ease of reporting (i.e., a score of 6 on the response scale is reported here as 60%).

Question items

Q1b. At what volume do you think people might put themselves at risk of hearing problems if they use a PSP regularly?

Q2. How loud do you usually listen to music through earphones or headphones?

Q3. How loud do you usually listen to music through speakers?

Q4. How loud do most of your friends usually listen to music through earphones or headphones?

Q5. How loud do most of your friends usually listen to music through speakers?

The questions were distributed throughout the questionnaire so as to minimise the likelihood of repeat effects from use of the scale.


  Results Top


The vast majority of the participants (90%) reported using a PSP and believed that PSP use may have the potential to pose a risk to hearing (97%). Of these, the mean volume level that participants believed to pose a risk to hearing was 79% (SD=16%, n=434). A one-way ANOVA showed that perceived risk volumes varied across the three age groups, F (2,427) = 13.63, P<0.001. Tukey post-hoc comparisons of the three groups showed significant differences between all three groups with Early High School students (M = 84%, 95% CI (81, 86)), reporting higher perceived risk volumes than the Senior High School group (M = 78%, 95% CI (76, 80)) and both reporting higher perceived risk volumes than the Young Adulthood group (M = 72%, 95% CI (69, 76)).

Participants' perceptions of listening levels (Qs 2-5) are shown in [Table 2] below, broken down by age.
Table 2: Participants perceptions of self and peers listening volumes, by age group

Click here to view


The listening condition (speakers or earphones/headphones) did not have a significant effect on volume rating for either condition for any of the three groups. Responses for both conditions were then averaged within each group to provide two single "Combination" scores, for both self-report volume levels and estimated peer volume levels [Table 3]. A one-way ANOVA showed no significant difference in self-reported listening volumes across the three groups, F (2,470) = 1.336, P=0.264. However, estimations of peers' listening volumes varied across the three age groups, F (2,460) = 3.387, P=0.035. Tukey post-hoc comparisons of the three groups showed a small but significant difference between Early High School students (M = 73%, 95% CI (70, 75)) reporting lower peer estimates than the Senior High School group (M = 76%, 95% CI (75, 78)).
Table 3: Comparison of perceptions of listening volumes for self-report and estimation of peer volumes, by group

Click here to view


Of specific interest to the current study was how participants within each group defined risky volume levels, their perceptions of their own listening volumes and the perceived social norms (beliefs about their friends' listening volumes). Paired sample t-tests were conducted to examine the comparisons between risk volumes, combination self-report volume levels and estimated peer volume levels within each group.

Combination self-report (Q2 and 3) and estimation of peer listening volume levels (Q4 and 5) were significantly lower than the levels perceived to carry a potential risk to hearing (Q1) for all groups: Early High school t(136) = 36.85, P<0.001 and t(138) = 49.44, P<0.001; Senior High School t(211) = 48.8, P<0.001, t(204) = 77.55, P<0.001; Young Adulthood t(77) = 31.24, P<0.001, t(77) = 53.87, P<0.001, respectively.

Furthermore, participants' estimation of peer listening levels were significantly higher than their self-reported listening levels for the Early High School group t(145) = 9.2, P<0.001; the Senior High School group t(229) = 11.76, P<0.001; and the Young Adulthood group t(84) = 8.519, P<0.001.


  Discussion Top


Overall, participants showed an awareness of the potential risk to hearing from PSP use, and indicated that they believed that this risk may be present at less than the maximum volume (specifically, 79%). Based on previous research estimates of maximum output of PSPs at 107 dB, a rating of 79% would represent a level of around 85 dB. This is a level that is potentially dangerous to hearing, and although the scale used was a relatively simple one, which did not include duration information or music genre preferences, it suggests that participants' estimate of risk can be considered reasonable.

One in five participants reported that they listened at volumes of 80% or more. This figure is in keeping with previous research examining measurements of users' actual PSP levels Williams, [5],[6] suggesting that the self-reported listening levels reported in this study are likely to be reasonably accurate estimations of personal listening habits. This finding is likely to be of interest to developers of hearing health promotion activities relating to PSP use as it suggests that a small proportion of users are likely to be at risk of hearing damage from their PSP listening behavior and, furthermore, self-reports may provide a reliable method of identifying those at risk.

For all groups, there was a significant difference between self-report and estimation of peers' listening levels, with nearly half the participants (46%) holding the belief that their friends listened at risky levels (i.e., 80% or more).

These findings are particularly interesting when considering that recruitment occurred within school environments where many participants' friends were also participants and, therefore, actually provided self-report estimates of their own behavior. Although a large proportion of participants suggested that their peers engaged in potentially risky behavior, only a small proportion of participants actually reported doing so. Participants therefore appear to hold inaccurate social norm beliefs, namely their misperceptions about the volume level at which their peers listen to music.

Overestimating peers' listening behaviors is likely to result in a corresponding overestimation of the "safety" of personal habits. This has the potential to reduce perceptions of personal susceptibility and decrease motivation to change personal listening behaviors. Individuals who believe that they are unlikely to be damaging their hearing by their current PSP listening behavior are unlikely to implement or even take notice of suggestions to reduce their risk. Furthermore, there is a chance that overestimation of peers' listening levels may lead individuals to attempt to emulate the behavior, increasing their own listening levels in order to meet the perceived social norm.

Thus, hearing health promotion activities may be more successful if such misperceptions are identified and addressed before the provision of more general preventative advice. These types of techniques have previously been implemented in health promotion campaigns, particularly with teenage audiences. For example, strategies have sought to correct misperceptions by providing individuals with accurate information about peer group behaviors and peer beliefs e.g., smoking, drug and alcohol consumption; Hansen; [12] Hansen and Graham. [13]

Limitations and future directions

This study only measured participants' perceptions of listening levels and did not measure actual PSP volumes or duration of use. It is therefore limited in its ability to determine the actual risk posed to participants by their PSP listening behavior. Regardless, it is the beliefs (accurate or otherwise) rather than actual behavior that are likely to be impacting on perceptions of susceptibility and thus remain a highly relevant area for investigation.

At any one time, a number of factors may influence participants real-world listening levels, including the model of music player and headphones/earplugs used, environmental background noise levels, music genre preference and even the fit of headphones to the ear canal. Information about these factors was not collected in the current study and so any subsequent impact they may have cannot be determined.

Because of the voluntary nature of the study, there is a possibility that those who participated were in fact "quiet users" and therefore not representative of the wider PSP listening population. However, as discussed, the range of self-report of listening volumes is in keeping with results from other random sample studies of PSP use, suggesting that the sample is representative.

Regardless, the results of this study have implications not just for leisure settings but also for work environments. Many workers in noisy industries are reliant on PPEs to reduce their noise exposure, with the result that successful hearing protection is highly dependent on individuals' choices to engage in the required behavior. However, motivating workers to use reduce their noise exposure has traditionally been a difficult task Waugh; [14] Westbrook, Hogan, Penny, and Legge. [15] Future work about occupational hearing protection behavior may benefit from closer examination of individuals' beliefs regarding their noise exposure and that of colleagues, to determine whether similar misperceptions exist that may be adversely influencing motivation.


  Conclusions Top


This study shows that while many young people seem to be aware of the potential risk to hearing from PSP use, they may be overestimating the listening behavior of their peers. Such a misperception has the potential to decrease feelings of susceptibility, leading to decreased motivation to engage in healthy hearing behaviors. Future hearing loss prevention campaigns (for both leisure and work environments) may benefit from consideration of social norm factors. In particular, campaigns that seek to identify and address inaccurate beliefs about social norm prior to delivery of prevention messages may be more successful in changing listening behavior and, ultimately, reducing the incidence of NIHL.


  Acknowledgments Top


Financial support for this research was provided through the Hearing Loss Prevention Program administered by the Office of Hearing Services, Australia, and the Commonwealth of Australia through the Cooperative Research Centres Program. The authors wish to thank Katrina Freeston, Monica Gibian and Jermy Pang for their ongoing assistance with the project.

 
  References Top

1.Keith SE, Michaud DS, Chiu V. Evaluating the maximum playback sound levels from portable digital audio players. J Acoust Soc Am 2008:123:4227-37.  Back to cited text no. 1
    
2.Portnuff CD, Fligor BJ. Output levels of portable digital music players. Paper presented at the Conference on Noise Induced Hearing Loss in Children at work and play;2006.  Back to cited text no. 2
    
3.Williams W, Purnell J. The statistical distribution of expected noise level output from commonly available personal stereo players. Acoust Aust 2010;38:119-22.  Back to cited text no. 3
    
4.Vogel I, Verschuure H, van der Ploeg CP, Brug J, Raat H. Adolescents and MP3 players: too many risks, Too few precautions. Pediatrics 2009;123:e953-8.  Back to cited text no. 4
[PUBMED]  [FULLTEXT]  
5.Williams W. Noise exposure levels from personal stereo use. Int J Audiol 2005;44:231-6.  Back to cited text no. 5
[PUBMED]    
6.Williams W. Trends in listening to personal stereos. Int J Audiol 2009;48:784-8.  Back to cited text no. 6
[PUBMED]  [FULLTEXT]  
7.Fligor BJ. Risk for Noise-induced hearing loss from use of portable media players: A summary of evidence through 2008. Perspect Audiol 2009;5:10-20.  Back to cited text no. 7
    
8.Australian Hearing. Binge Listening: Is exposure to leisure noise causing hearing loss in young Australians? Hearing health report, Australian Hearing, Sydney, NSW; 2010.  Back to cited text no. 8
    
9.Ajzen I. From intentions to actions: A theory of planned behaviour. In: Julius K, Beckmann J, editors. Action Control: From Cognition to Behaviour. New York: Springer-Verlag; 1985. p. 10-39.  Back to cited text no. 9
    
10.Ajzen I. The theory of planned behaviour. Organ Behav Hum Decis Process 1991:50:179-211.  Back to cited text no. 10
    
11.Carter L. Prevalence of hearing loss and its relationship to leisure-sound exposure: iHEAR. Paper presented at the audiology Australia XIX National Conference; 2010.  Back to cited text no. 11
    
12.Hansen WB. A social ecology theory of alcohol and drug use prevention among college and university students. In: Baker K, Maas J, McAuliffe A, Wojdyslawski S, Zweig K, editors. Designing alcohol and other drug prevention programs in higher education: Bringing theory into practice. Newport, MA: The higher education center for alcohol and other drug prevention; 1997. p. 155-75.  Back to cited text no. 12
    
13.Hansen WB, Graham JW. Preventing alcohol, marijuana, and cigarette use among adolescents: Peer pressure resistance training versus establishing conservative norms. Prev Med 1991;20:414-30.  Back to cited text no. 13
[PUBMED]    
14.Waugh D. Do hearing conservation programs really work? Brisbane: Proceedings of the National Seminar on Noise Management in the Workplace; 1993.  Back to cited text no. 14
    
15.Westbrook MT, Hogan A, Penny M, Legge V. Workers' reactions to their noise-induced hearing loss: Acknowledgement versus avoidance. J Occupational Health Safety- Aust NZ, 1992;8:237-42.  Back to cited text no. 15
    

Top
Correspondence Address:
Megan Gilliver
126 Greville St, Chatswood
Australia
Login to access the Email id

Source of Support: The project received funding from the Australian Government's Office of Hearing Services under its Hearing Loss Prevention Program, and from The Hearing CRC, Conflict of Interest: None


DOI: 10.4103/1463-1741.95131

Rights and Permissions


    Figures

  [Figure 1]
 
 
    Tables

  [Table 1], [Table 2], [Table 3]

This article has been cited by
1 Hearing loss prevention at loud music events via real-time visuo-haptic feedback
Luca Turchet, Simone Luiten, Tjebbe Treub, Marloes van der Burgt, Costanza Siani, Alberto Boem
Journal on Multimodal User Interfaces. 2023;
[Pubmed] | [DOI]
2 Prioritizing the target audience for a hearing awareness campaign in Australia using the TARPARE model
Simon Alperstein, Elizabeth Francis Beach
Health Promotion International. 2022;
[Pubmed] | [DOI]
3 Sound thoughts: How understanding the teenage brain may help us look after their ears
Jermy Pang, Megan Gilliver
Frontiers in Integrative Neuroscience. 2022; 16
[Pubmed] | [DOI]
4 A smartphone application to objectively monitor music listening habits in adolescents
Danique E. Paping, Jantien L. Vroegop, Simone P. C. Koenraads, Carlijn M.P. le Clercq, André Goedegebure, Robert J. Baatenburg de Jong, Marc P. van der Schroeff
Journal of Otolaryngology - Head & Neck Surgery. 2021; 50(1)
[Pubmed] | [DOI]
5 “Know Your Noise” Helps Young People Understand Risks Associated with Recreational Noise Exposure and Take Action to Prevent Hearing Damage
Megan Gilliver, Elizabeth Francis Beach
Journal of Consumer Health on the Internet. 2021; 25(4): 350
[Pubmed] | [DOI]
6 The effects of different styles of musical auditory stimulation on cardiac autonomic regulation in healthy women
Roque, A.L. and Valenti, V.E. and Guida, H.L. and Campos, M.F. and Knap, A. and Vanderlei, L.C.M. and Ferreira, C. and De Abreu, L.C.
Noise and Health. 2013; 15(65): 281-287
[Pubmed]
7 Hearing protectors and people
Williams, W.
41st International Congress and Exposition on Noise Control Engineering 2012, INTER-NOISE 2012. 2012; 5
[Pubmed]



 

Top