Article Access Statistics | | Viewed | 7422 | | Printed | 242 | | Emailed | 2 | | PDF Downloaded | 23 | | Comments | [Add] | | Cited by others | 17 | |
|

|
|
|
Year : 2015
| Volume
: 17 | Issue : 75 | Page
: 83-89 |
|
Tualang honey improves memory performance and decreases depressive-like behavior in rats exposed to loud noise stress |
|
Khairunnuur Fairuz Azman1, Rahimah Zakaria1, CheBadariah AbdAziz1, Zahiruddin Othman1, Badriya Al-Rahbi2
1 Department of Physiology, School of Medical Sciences, Universiti Sains Malaysia, Malaysia 2 Institute of Health Sciences, Muscat, Oman
Click here for correspondence address
and email
Date of Web Publication | 16-Mar-2015 |
|
|
 |
|
Recent evidence has exhibited dietary influence on the manifestation of different types of behavior induced by stressor tasks. The present study examined the effects of Tualang honey supplement administered with the goal of preventing or attenuating the occurrence of stress-related behaviors in male rats subjected to noise stress. Forty-eight adult male rats were randomly divided into the following four groups: i) nonstressed with vehicle, ii) nonstressed with Tualang honey, iii) stressed with vehicle, and iv) stressed with honey. The supplement was given once daily via oral gavage at 0.2 g/kg body weight. Two types of behavioral tests were performed, namely, the novel object recognition test to evaluate working memory and the forced swimming test to evaluate depressive-like behavior. Data were analyzed by a two-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) using IBM SPSS 18.0. It was observed that the rats subjected to noise stress expressed higher levels of depressive-like behavior and lower memory functions compared to the unexposed control rats. In addition, our results indicated that the supplementation regimen successfully counteracted the effects of noise stress. The forced swimming test indicated that climbing and swimming times were significantly increased and immobility times significantly decreased in honey-supplemented rats, thereby demonstrating an antidepressant-like effect. Furthermore, cognitive function was shown to be intensely affected by noise stress, but the effects were counteracted by the honey supplement. These findings suggest that subchronic exposure to noise stress induces depressive-like behavior and reduces cognitive functions, and that these effects can be attenuated by Tualang honey supplementation. This warrants further studies to examine the role of Tulang honey in mediating such effects. Keywords: Antioxidants, behavior, brain, cognitive, depression
How to cite this article: Azman KF, Zakaria R, AbdAziz C, Othman Z, Al-Rahbi B. Tualang honey improves memory performance and decreases depressive-like behavior in rats exposed to loud noise stress. Noise Health 2015;17:83-9 |
How to cite this URL: Azman KF, Zakaria R, AbdAziz C, Othman Z, Al-Rahbi B. Tualang honey improves memory performance and decreases depressive-like behavior in rats exposed to loud noise stress. Noise Health [serial online] 2015 [cited 2023 Jun 7];17:83-9. Available from: https://www.noiseandhealth.org/text.asp?2015/17/75/83/153388 |
Introduction | |  |
Numerous reports have shown that noise exposure affects cognitive performance and induces behavioral changes. Increased noise levels have been associated with decreases in intentional, incidental, and recognition memory in children, [1],[2],[3] a result that has been paralleled in rats. [4],[5],[6] Chronic noise exposure in industrial workers and individuals living near major transportation routes has been associated with depression and feelings of aggression. [7],[8] Noise may also be fear-inducing, as evidenced by a more prominent tonic immobility response in noise-stressed hens. [9],[10]
Tualang honey is a multifloral honey produced by the rock bee species (Apis dorsata) that builds hives high up in the branches of the Tualang tree (Kompassia excelsa). Recent studies revealed that the consumption of honey may reduce anxiety and improve spatial memory in middle-aged rats. [11] It is also reported that honey is used in natural preventive therapies for both cognitive decline and dementia, as it possesses antioxidant properties and enhances the brain's cholinergic system and circulation. [12] In closely related studies, it was demonstrated that Tualang honey was able to improve memory performance in stressed ovariectomized (OVX) rats [13] and postmenopausal women. [14] To our knowledge, no previous research had been done regarding the effects of Tualang honey on cognitive functions and depression in rats exposed to loud noise stress. Therefore, this study aimed to evaluate the efficacy of Tualang honey in improving cognitive functions and reducing depressive symptoms.
Materials and Methods | |  |
Experimental animals
Forty-eight male Sprague-Dawley rats (2 months old, weighing 200-250 g) were used in this study. The animals were housed under a reversed 12-h light/dark cycle (lights off at 8 AM) at a consistent room temperature of ±27°C, with free access to commercial rat chow (Gold Coin Ltd., Kuala Lumpur, Malaysia) and water. Rats were allowed to acclimatize to the holding room for 24 h before the behavioral procedures. The procedures in this study were approved by the Animal Ethics Committee of the Universiti Sains Malaysia (USM/Animal Ethics Approval/2013(85)(444).
Honey supplement
The Tualang honey used was from a single batch of honey supplied by the Federal Agricultural Marketing Authorities (FAMA), Malaysia. The honey was filtered by FAMA to remove solid particles, concentrated in an oven at 40°C, and evaporated to achieve a water content of about 20%. It was then subjected to γ irradiation at 25 kGy at SterilGamma (M) Sdn. Bhd. (Selangor, Malaysia) for sterilization and bottled 230 g per jar. The final concentration of the bottled Tualang honey was 1.3 g/mL. Tualang honey (0.2 g/kg body weight) was dissolved in distilled water and given once a day via oral gavage over a period of 35 days. The dissolution of Tualang honey was freshly done before the administration. The control groups received an identical volume of distilled water (vehicle) as placebo.
Experimental design
The animals were randomly assigned to the following groups (n = 12):
- Nonstressed with vehicle,
- Nonstressed with Tualang honey,
- Stressed with vehicle, and
- Stressed with Tualang honey.
As illustrated in [Figure 1], honey supplementation was started on day 1 and continued for a period of 35 days. On days 22-35, the noise stress procedure was implemented. The behavior tests were conducted following the final day of stress exposure. The novel object recognition tests were conducted from day 36 to day 38, followed by the forced swimming test from day 39 to day 41. The animals were killed by decapitation upon completion of the behavioral tests. Individual body weights were recorded weekly using electrical balance.
Noise stress exposure
The animals of the test groups (iii and iv) were exposed to white noise for 4 h (9 AM-1 PM) daily for 14 days. Noise was recorded from the generator and amplified by speakers in a separate room. Speakers were located 30 cm above the cages. The noise level was set at 100 dB(A) and intensity was measured by sound level meter CENTER 325 (range: 80-130 dB(A); accuracy: +1.5 dB(A); made in Taiwan). Sound levels were verified at the center of the cage before each exposure and varied by less than 1 dB(A) in the space the cage occupied. The control groups were kept in the same room for the same period of time without switching on the noise.
Behavioral tests
All the behavioral tests were conducted after the noise stress procedure. Two types of behavioral tests were performed, namely, the novel object recognition test to evaluate working memory and the forced swimming test to evaluate depressive-like behavior. The behavioral tests were carried out in a separate room that was ventilated, soundproof, and maintained at a constant temperature (±27°C). The animals were brought into the test room 1 h before the tests began to minimize the arousal caused by the transference. All behavioral tests were performed during the active period of the animals (dark phase) between 9 AM and 2 PM. All the animals were tested in a random order. The trained observer remained blind to the treatment group of the rats until scoring was completed.
Novel object recognition test
The test employed was similar to that described elsewhere. [15] The test was conducted in an open box made of transparent plastic 60 cm × 60 cm × 30 cm. Training sessions were conducted on two successive days, with the animals allowed to explore the arena for 10 min each day. In each training session, two identical sample objects were placed in the field in a symmetrical pattern about 10 cm away from the wall. The objects discriminated were made of plastic and varied in shape and color. Between tests, the objects were cleaned with a 10% ethanol solution to mask any olfactory cues. All combinations and locations (left and right) of the objects were alternated in order to prevent potential bias due to preferences for particular locations or objects.
After the two successive training sessions, testing/retention sessions were conducted. The retention sessions consisted of two sessions, for short-term memory and long-term memory, in which the retention intervals for short-term memory and long-term memory were 2 h and 24 h, respectively, after the last training session. In the retention session, rats were placed back in the same field, where one of the familiar objects used in the training session was replaced by a novel object, and the rats were allowed to explore for 5 min. The times spent exploring each object were video-recorded. Exploration of an object was defined as directing the nose toward the object at a distance of 2 cm or less. Climbing and leaning on an object was not considered exploratory behavior.
The total exploration time of the familiar and novel objects were used to calculate discrimination index. The discrimination index is an index of measures of discrimination between the familiar and the novel objects corrected for exploratory activity. It is calculated as: (time spent on novel object − time spent on familiar object)/(time spent on novel obejct + time spent on familiar object). The discrimination index can range from −1 to 1, with −1 indicating complete preference for the familiar object, 0 indicating no preference for either object, and 1 indicating complete preference for the novel object.
Forced swimming test
The test employed was similar to that described elsewhere, [16],[17] with slight modification. The alterations consisted of increasing the water depth from 15-18 cm in the original to a depth of 30 cm and moving from a cumulative timing measure to a time-sampling technique wherein the predominant behavior over each 5-sec period of the 300-s test was rated. [18] All the animals were individually placed in a transparent plastic cylinder (40 cm in height × 18 cm in diameter) filled with water (25-27°C) to a level of 30 cm. The experimental session consisted of two trials: The conditioning trial and the test trial. [17] During the conditioning trial, the rats were placed in the water-filled cylinder for 15 min. After the trial, the rats were dried and placed in a warm cage with paper towels for 10-15 min before being returned to their home cages. Twenty-four hours later, the animals were placed again in the cylinder for a 5-min test session, that is, the test trial.
The test sessions were videotaped for subsequent quantitative behavioral analysis. The frequency and/or total duration was calculated for each of the predominant behaviors: Climbing (intense movements with all four limbs, with the two forepaws breaking the surface of the water and being directed against the walls of the cylinder); swimming (rigorous horizontal movements throughout all radiuses of the cylinder); and immobile (the animal remaining in water with all four limbs motionless except for the occasional alternate movement of paws and tail necessary to prevent sinking and to keep the head/nose above water). The water was changed before the next animal was placed in the water tank.
Statistical analysis
The data obtained were analyzed using a two-way analysis of variance (ANOVA), with stress treatment (no noise vs loud noise) and honey treatment (control vs honey) as fixed factors. All analyses were performed using IBM SPSS version 18. Statistical data were reported as mean ± SEM, and a result was deemed to be statistically significant if P < 0.05.
Results | |  |
Novel object recognition test
The results are depicted in [Figure 2]. The univariate ANOVA revealed the significant main effects of stress on short-term memory [F(1,46) = 6.77, P < 0.05] and long-term memory [F(1,46) = 40.25, P < 0.01]. The loud noise stress-exposed rats exhibited a significantly lower mean discrimination index in both short-term and long-term memory compared to nonexposed rats, which indicates deteriorated memory. Interestingly, there were significant main effects of honey treatment on short-term memory [F(1,46 = 18.45, P < 0.01] and long-term memory [F(1,46 = 34.7, P < 0.01]. Rats given the honey supplement showed a significantly higher mean discrimination index in both short-term and long-term memory compared to the control rats, indicating better memory performance. However, there was no significant interaction effect between stress and honey treatment. | Figure 2: The effects of loud noise stress and honey supplementation on discrimination index of (a) short-term memory and (b) long-term memory in the novel object recognition test. Data are displayed as mean ± SEM. Signifi cant main effects of loud noise stress (#, P < 0.05; ##, P < 0.01) and honey treatment (**, P < 0.01)
Click here to view |
Forced swimming test
The results were depicted in [Figure 3]. The univariate ANOVA revealed the significant main effects of stress on durations of climbing [F(1,43) = 5.64, P < 0.01], swimming [F(1,43) = 8.09, P < 0.01], and immobility [F(1,45) = 14.55, P < 0.01]. The rats exposed to loud noise stress exhibited significantly lower climbing and swimming durations and higher immobility durations compared to nonexposed rats, which indicates depressive-like symptoms. In addition, the analyses revealed significant main effects of honey treatment on durations of climbing [F(1,43 = 6.27, P < 0.01], swimming [F(1,43 = 11.32, P < 0.01], and immobility [F(1,45 = 19.65, P < 0.01]. Rats treated with the honey supplement showed significantly a higher duration of climbing and swimming and significantly lower duration of immobility, which reflected honey's antidepressant-like effects. However, there was no significant interaction effect between stress and honey treatment.  | Figure 3: The effects of loud noise stress and honey supplementation on durations of (a) climbing, (b) swimming and (c) immobility behavior in the forced swimming test. Data are displayed as mean ± SEM. Signifi cant main effects of loud noise stress (#, P < 0.05; ##, P < 0.01) and honey treatment (*, P < 0.05; **, P < 0.01)
Click here to view |
Discussion | |  |
Stress responses are considered to play an important role in mental disorders. [19] Evans et al., (1995) [20] pointed out that chronic noise exposure is associated with cognitive as well as affective disorders of psychological stress. According to the definition of chronic stress proposed by Burchfield (1979), [21] chronic stress can be interpreted as a succession of repeated acute stressors. Two animal behavior models, the novel object recognition test, and the forced swimming test were selected in this study to investigate the effects of noise stress on cognitive performance and depressive-like behavior in rats.
The novel object recognition test has been shown to be an effective model for assessing learning and memory across species, including mice, [22] hamsters, [23] rats, [24] and pigs. [25] It is widely accepted that noise is a stressful environmental stimulus, and stress has been previously shown to impair cognition, such as the acquisition of memory, consolidation, and recall. [26],[27] In the present study, it was observed that the noise-exposed rats demonstrated significantly lower cognitive performance compared to the nonexposed groups. The decrease of the mean discrimination index in both short-term and long-term memory sessions of the rats exposed to noise compared to control reflected a memory deficit in the recognition task. The adverse effects of noise exposure observed on spatial and recognition memory are in agreement with the findings of other researchers. [28],[29],[30]
The hippocampus plays an important role in spatial memory for both humans and rodents. [31],[32] The hippocampus is also connected to the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis and is particularly susceptible to stress. [33],[34] With prolonged chronic stress, the HPA axis is hyperactivated, resulting in the release of adrenocorticotropic hormone (ACTH) and corticosterone, thus resulting in structural changes, cell atrophy, and neuronal loss in the hippocampus. [33],[35],[36] Thus, it is postulated that the deterioration of memory noted in the noise-exposed rats is due to stress, which then activates the HPA axis, triggers the release of ACTH and corticosterone levels, and consequently causes hippocampal neurodegeneration.
The HPA axis dysregulation has also been closely linked to the maintenance and triggering of depression. [37] The forced swimming test as originally reported by Porsolt and colleagues [16],[17] has become the most widely used model for assessing antidepressant-like activity in rats. The test is based on the observation that when rats are exposed to water from which there is no escape, after initial intense escape-directed behavior, such as swimming and climbing, they stop struggling and show passive, immobile behavior. The immobile behavior is believed to reflect either a failure to persist in escape-directed behavior after stress (i.e., behavioral despair) or the development of passive behavior that disengages the animal from active forms of stress coping. [38],[39],[40],[41] In the current study, the rats exposed to noise demonstrated a significant decrease in climbing and swimming durations as well as increased immobility duration, indicating depressive-like behavior. These findings are consistent with a previous study where exposure to loud noise for 15 days caused a longer immobility duration. [42] Brain neurotransmitters play an essential role in the pathophysiology of depression. Noise stress has been shown to diminish dopamine levels after 15 days of noise exposure. [43] Hence, it is suggested that noise stress causes reduced dopamine levels, thus resulting in diminished dopamine neurotransmission, and further promotes depressive disorders.
It is also important to note that overexposure to intense sound may cause the destruction of cochlear hair cells, damage to the mechanosensory hair bundles, and loss of spiral ganglion cells and the cell bodies of the cochlear afferent neurons. [44],[45] This noise-induced peripheral neurodegeneration may lead to changes in brainstem circuitry and cortical reorganization, and further result in abnormal auditory behavior, including tinnitus- and hyperacusis-like behavior. [46] Hence, it is also possible that the inner ear damage caused by overexposure to loud noise may have caused the behavioral changes seen in the rats. However, so far, there is no evidence suggesting a relationship between noise-induced inner ear damage or peripheral neurodegeneration and behavioral changes related to memory and depression. Therefore, further studies are required to examine the ear condition of the noise-exposed rats followed by observation of the behavioral changes.
Interestingly, this study revealed the potential of Tualang honey in improving memory and reducing depressive symptoms induced by loud noise stress. In the novel object recognition test, it was observed that the rats supplemented with Tualang honey exhibited significantly higher memory scores compared to the control rats. In addition, the rats demonstrated significantly higher duration of climbing and swimming and a lower duration of immobility in the forced swimming test.
Previously, it was reported that Tualang honey causes a significant reduction in ACTH and corticosterone levels, as well as depressive-like behavior in OVX rats exposed to social instability stress. [47] Apart from that, Tualang honey-treated stressed OVX rats exhibited elevation in brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) concentration. [47] Decreased expression of BDNF has been proved to contribute to hippocampal atrophy and neuronal loss in experimental animals [48] and was further evidenced by decreased hippocampal volume in depressed patients. [49],[50] Hence, it is suggested that Tualang honey exhibits its antidepressive-like effects via restoration of HPA axis and enhancement of brain BDNF concentration. As Tualang honey is a phytoestrogen rich in flavonoids, it is possible that the mechanisms of antidepressive-like actions are similar to other phytochemical foods rich in flavonoids, such as green tea, blueberry, and Ginkgo biloba, which have been shown to increase hippocampal BDNF levels. [51],[52],[53],[54],[55]
It is also assumed that the improvement in memory and reduction of depressive symptoms in the stress-induced rats caused by Tualang honey are due to its antioxidant capacity, which is attributed to the aforementioned flavonoid contents. Honey has been reported to possess a high flavonoid content (of flavonoids such as quercetin, luteolin, kaempferol, apigenin, chrysin, and galangin), which ranges 60-460 μg/100 g of honey. [56] Other types of antioxidants also present in honey include both enzymatic (catalase, glucose oxidase, and peroxidase) and nonenzymatic substances (ascorbic acid, α-tocopherol, carotenoids, amino acids, proteins, Maillard reaction products, and phenolic acids). [57],[58],[59] It is possible that the antioxidant content of the honey may have contributed to the decrease of depressive-like behavior and improved the recognition memory of these rats. This would be in keeping with studies that have demonstrated that dietary antioxidants improve cognitive performance in clinical studies [60],[61] as well as in animals. [62],[63],[64] In addition, studies by Chepulis et al. (2009) [11] reported that honey-fed rats exhibited better memory performance and decreased anxiety, compared to sucrose-fed rats. This suggests that the better performance seen in the honey-fed rats was not due to the sugar content alone but may have involved other components, possibly antioxidants, of the honey.
Conclusion | |  |
The data in this study strongly suggest a negative role for loud noise stress on the cognitive performance and depressive symptoms of male rats. Moreover, it was shown that daily supplementation with Tualang honey may reverse the damage caused by stress exposure. Due to the promising effects of Tualang honey, further studies are warranted to identify the possible mechanism of its action.
References | |  |
1. | Lercher P, Evans GW, Meis M. Ambient noise and cognitive processes among primary schoolchildren. Environ Behav 2003;35:725-35. |
2. | Evans GW, Bullinger M, Hygge S. Chronic noise exposure and physiological response: A prospective study of children living under environmental stress. Psychol Sci 1998;9:75-7. |
3. | Stansfeld SA, Berglund B, Clark C, Lopez-Barrio I, Fischer P, Ohrström E, et al.; RANCH Study Team. Aircraft and road traffic noise and children′s cognition and health: A cross-national study. Lancet 2005;365:1942-9. |
4. | Cui B, Wu M, She X. Effects of chronic noise exposure on spatial learning and memory of rats in relation to neurotransmitters and NMDAR2B alteration in the hippocampus. J Occup Health 2009;51:152-8. |
5. | Rabat A. Extra-auditory effects of noise in laboratory animals: The relationship between noise and sleep. J Am Assoc Lab Anim Sci 2007;46:35-41. |
6. | Uygur EE, Arslan M. Effects of chronic stress on cognitive functions and anxiety related behaviors in rats. Acta Physiol Hung 2010;97:297-306. |
7. | Ising H, Kruppa B. Health effects caused by noise: Evidence in the literature from the past 25 years. Noise Health 2004;6:5-13.  [ PUBMED] |
8. | Stansfeld SA, Matheson MP. Noise pollution: Non-auditory effects on health. Br Med Bull 2003;68:243-57. |
9. | Campo JL, Gil MG, Dávila SG. Effects of specific noise and music stimuli on stress and fear levels of laying hens of several breeds. App Anim Behav Sci 2005;91:75-84. |
10. | Chloupek P, Voslářová E, Chloupek J, Bedáňová I, Pištěková V, Večerek V. Stress in broiler chickens due to acute noise exposure. Acta Vet Brno 2009;78:93-8. |
11. | Chepulis LM, Starkey NJ, Waas JR, Molan PC. The effects of long-term honey, sucrose or sugar-free diets on memory and anxiety in rats. Physiol Behav 2009;97:359-68. |
12. | |
13. | Al-Rahbi B, Zakaria R, Othman Z, Hassan A, Mohd Ismail ZI, Muthuraju S. Tualang honey supplement improves memory performance and hippocampal morphology in stressed overiectomized rats. Acta Histochem 2014;116:79-88. |
14. | Othman Z, Shafin N, Zakaria R, Hussain NH, Mohammad WM. Improvement in immediate memory after 16 weeks of tualang honey (Agro Mas) supplement in healthy postmenopausal women. Menopause 2011;18:1219-24. |
15. | Ennaceur A, Delacour J. A new one-trial test for neurobiological studies of memory in rats. 1: Behavioral data. Behav Brain Res 1988;31:47-59. |
16. | Porsolt R, Bertin A, Jalfre M. Behavioral despair in mice: A primary screening test for antidepressants. Arch Int Pharmacodyn Ther 1977;229:327-36. |
17. | Porsolt RD, Anton G, Blavet N, Jalfre M. Behavioural despair in rats: A new model sensitive to antidepressant treatments. Eur J Pharmacol 1978;47:379-91.  [ PUBMED] |
18. | Slattery DA, Desrayaud S, Cryan JF. GABAB receptor antagonist-mediated antidepressant-like behavior is serotonin-dependent. J Pharmacol Exp Ther 2005;312:290-6. |
19. | McEwen BS. Allostasis and allostatic load: Implications for neuropsychopharmacology. Neuropsychopharmacology 2000;22:108-24. |
20. | Evans GW, Hygge S, Bullinger M. Chronic noise and psychological stress. Psychol Sci 1995;6:333-8. |
21. | Burchfield SR. The stress response: A new perspective. Psychosom Med 1979;41:661-72.  [ PUBMED] |
22. | Sik A, van Nieuwehuyzen P, Prickaerts J, Blokland A. Performance of different mouse strains in an object recognition task. Behav Brain Res 2003;147:49-54. |
23. | Palchykova S, Crestani F, Meerlo P, Tobler I. Sleep deprivation and daily torpor impair object recognition in Djungarian hamsters. Physiol Behav 2006;87:144-53. |
24. | Blokland A, Prickaerts J, Honig W, de Vente J. State-dependent impairment in object recognition after hippocampal NOS inhibition. Neuroreport 1998;9:4205-8. |
25. | Moustgaard A, Lind NM, Hemmingsen R, Hansen AK. Spontaneous object recognition in the Göttingen minipig. Neural Plast 2002;9:255-9. |
26. | Arnsten AF, Goldman-Rakic PS. Noise stress impairs prefrontal cortical cognitive function in monkeys: Evidence for a hyperdopaminergic mechanism. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1998;55:362-8. |
27. | Lupien SJ, McEwen BS. The acute effects of corticosteroids on cognition: Integration of animal and human model studies. Brain Res Brain Res Rev 1997;24:1-27. |
28. | Chengzhi C, Yan T, Xuejun J, Xiang L, Youbin Q, Baijie T. Recovery of chronic noise exposure induced spatial learning and memory deficits in young male Sprague-Dawley rats. J Occup Health 2011;53:157-63. |
29. | Haider S, Naqvi F, Batool Z, Tabassum S, Perveen T, Saleem S, et al. Decreased hippocampal 5-HT and DA levels following sub-chronic exposure to noise stress: Impairment in both spatial and recognition memory in male rats. Sci Pharm 2012;80:1001-11. |
30. | Manikandan S, Padma MK, Srikumar R, Jeya Parthasarathy N, Muthuvel A, Sheela Devi R. Effects of chronic noise stress on spatial memory of rats in relation to neuronal dendritic alteration and free radical-imbalance in hippocampus and medial prefrontal cortex. Neurosci Lett 2006;399:17-22. |
31. | O′Keefe J. Do hippocampal pyramidal cells signal non-spatial as well as spatial information? Hippocampus 1999;9:352-64. |
32. | Burgess N, Maguire EA, O′Keefe J. The human hippocampus and spatial and episodic memory. Neuron 2002;35:625-41. |
33. | Lara DR, Pinto O, Akiskal K, Akiskal HS. Toward an integrative model of the spectrum of mood, behavioral and personality disorders based on fear and anger traits: I. Clinical implications. J Affect Disord 2006;94:67-87. |
34. | McGirr A, Diaconu G, Berlim MT, Pruessner JC, Sablé R, Cabot S, et al. Dysregulation of the sympathetic nervous system, hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis and executive function in individuals at risk for suicide. J Psychiatry Neurosci 2010;35:399-408. |
35. | Sapolsky RM. Glucocorticoids, stress, and their adverse neurological effects: Relevance to aging. Exp Gerontol 1999;34:721-32. |
36. | McEwen BS. Plasticity of the hippocampus: Adaptation to chronic stress and allostatic load. Ann N Y Acad Sci 2001;933:265-77. |
37. | Checkley S. The neuroendocrinology of depression and chronic stress. Br Med Bull 1996;52:597-617. |
38. | Lucki I. The forced swimming test as a model for core and component behavioral effects of antidepressant drugs. Behav Pharmacol 1997;8:523-32. |
39. | Cryan JF, Markou A, Lucki I. Assessing antidepressant activity in rodents: Recent developments and future needs. Trends Pharmacol Sci 2002;23:238-45. |
40. | Cryan JF, Mombereau C. In search of a depressed mouse: Utility of models for studying depression-related behavior in genetically modified mice. Mol Psychiatry 2004;9:326-57. |
41. | Slattery DA, Desrayaud S, Cryan JF. GABAB receptor antagonist-mediated antidepressant-like behavior is serotonin-dependent. J Pharmacol Exp Ther 2005;312:290-6. |
42. | Bulduk S, Canbeyli R. Effect of inescapable tones on behavioral despair in Wistar rats. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry 2004;28:471-5. |
43. | Ravindran R, Rathinasamy SD, Samson J, Senthilvelan M. Noise-stress-induced brain neurotransmitter changes and the effect of Ocimum sanctum (Linn) treatment in albino rats. J Pharmacol Sci 2005;98:354-60. |
44. | Kujawa SG, Liberman MC. Adding insult to injury: Cochlear nerve degeneration after "temporary" noise-induced hearing loss. J Neurosci 2009;29:14077-85. |
45. | Liberman MC, Dodds LW. Single-neuron labeling and chronic cochlear pathology. III. Stereocilia damage and alterations of threshold tuning curves. Hear Res 1984;16:55-74. |
46. | Hickox AE, Liberman MC. Is noise-induced cochlear neuropathy key to the generation of hyperacusis or tinnitus? J Neurophysiol 2014;111:552-64. |
47. | Al-Rahbi B, Zakaria R, Othman Z, Hassan A, Ahmad AH. Enhancement of BDNF concentration and restoration of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis accompany reduced depressive-like behavior in stressed ovariectomised rats treated with either Tualang honey or estrogen. Sci World J 2014;2014:310821. |
48. | McEwen BS. Stress and hippocampal plasticity. Ann Rev Neurosci 1999;22:105-22. |
49. | Duman RS, Malberg J, Nakagawa S. Regulation of adult neurogenesis by psychotropic drugs and stress. J Pharmacol Exp Ther 2001;299:401-7. |
50. | Manji HK, Drevets WC, Charney DS. The cellular neurobiology of depression. Nat Med 2001;7:541-7. |
51. | Hou Y, Aboukhatwa MA, Lei DL, Manaye K, Khan I, Luo Y. Anti-depressant natural flavonols modulate BDNF and beta amyloid in neurons and hippocampus of double TgAD mice. Neuropharmacol 2010;58:911-20. |
52. | Li Q, Zhao HF, Zhang ZF, Liu ZG, Pei XR, Wang JB, et al. Long-term administration of green tea catechins prevents age-related spatial learning and memory decline in C57BL/6 J mice by regulating hippocampal cyclic amp-response element binding protein signaling cascade. Neuroscience 2009;159:1208-15. |
53. | Li Q, Zhao HF, Zhang ZF, Liu ZG, Pei XR, Wang JB, et al. Long-term green tea catechin administration prevents spatial learning and memory impairment in senescence-accelerated mouse prone-8 mice by decreasing Abeta1-42 oligomers and upregulating synaptic plasticity-related proteins in the hippocampus. Neuroscience 2009;163:741-9. |
54. | Williams CM, El Mohsen MA, Vauzour D, Rendeiro C, Butler LT, Ellis JA, et al. Blueberry-induced changes in spatial working memory correlate with changes in hippocampal CREB phosphorylation and brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) levels. Free Radic Biol Med 2008;45:295-305. |
55. | Rendeiro C, Vauzour D, Rattray M, Waffo-Téguo P, Mérillon JM, Butler LT, et al. Dietary levels of pure flavonoids improve spatial memory performance and increase hippocampal brain-derived neurotrophic factor. PloS one 2013;8:e63535. |
56. | Bogdanov S, Jurendic T, Sieber R, Gallmann P. Honey for nutrition and health: A review. J Am Coll Nutr 2008;27:677-89. |
57. | Al ML, Daniel D, Moise A, Bobis O, Laslo L, Bogdanov S. Physico-chemical and bioactive properties of different floral origin honeys from Romania. Food Chem 2009;112:863-7. |
58. | Ferreira IC, Aires E, Barreira JC, Estevinho LM. Antioxidant activity of Portuguese honey samples: Different contributions of the entire honey and phenolic extract. Food Chem 2009;114:1438-43. |
59. | Gheldof N, Engeseth NJ. Antioxidant capacity of honeys from various floral sources based on the determination of oxygen radical absorbance capacity and inhibition of in vitro lipoprotein oxidation in human serum samples. J Agric Food Chem 2002;50:3050-5. |
60. | Jama JW, Launer LJ, Witteman JC, den Breeijen JH, Breteler MM, Grobbee DE, et al. Dietary antioxidants and cognitive function in a population-based sample of older persons. The Rotterdam Study. Am J Epidemiol 1996;144:275-80. |
61. | Wengreen HJ, Munger RG, Corcoran CD, Zandi P, Hayden KM, Fotuhi M, et al. Antioxidant intake and cognitive function of elderly men and women: The Cache County Study. J Nutr Health Aging 2007;11:230-7. |
62. | Abreu RV, Silva-Oliveira EM, Moraes MF, Pereira GS, Moraes-Santos T. Chronic coffee and caffeine ingestion effects on the cognitive function and antioxidant system of rat brains. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 2011;99:659-64. |
63. | Cotman CW, Head E, Muggenburg BA, Zicker S, Milgram NW. Brain aging in the canine: A diet enriched in antioxidants reduces cognitive dysfunction. Neurobiol Aging 2002;23:809-18. |
64. | Fahnestock M, Marchese M, Head E, Pop V, Michalski B, Milgram WN, et al. BDNF increases with behavioral enrichment and an antioxidant diet in the aged dog. Neurobiol Aging 2012;33:546-54. |

Correspondence Address: Khairunnuur Fairuz Azman Department of Physiology, School of Medical Sciences, Health Campus, Universiti Sains Malaysia, Kubang Kerian - 16150, Kota Bharu, Kelantan Malaysia
 Source of Support: This project was supported by short-term grant of Universiti Sains Malaysia (304/PPSP/61313056) and grant from Ministry of Health of Oman (304/PPSP/6150117/M137)., Conflict of Interest: None  | Check |
DOI: 10.4103/1463-1741.153388

[Figure 1], [Figure 2], [Figure 3] |
|
This article has been cited by | 1 |
Honey on brain health: A promising brain booster |
|
| Nurul Ashykin Zamri, Nurhafizah Ghani, Che Aishah Nazariah Ismail, Rahimah Zakaria, Nazlahshaniza Shafin | | Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience. 2023; 14 | | [Pubmed] | [DOI] | | 2 |
Physicochemical Characteristics and Bioactive Compounds of Different Types of Honey and Their Biological and Therapeutic Properties: A Comprehensive Review |
|
| Mohammad A. Al-Kafaween, Mohammad Alwahsh, Abu Bakar Mohd Hilmi, Dina H. Abulebdah | | Antibiotics. 2023; 12(2): 337 | | [Pubmed] | [DOI] | | 3 |
Honey and Alzheimer’s Disease—Current Understanding and Future Prospects |
|
| Ammara Shaikh, Fairus Ahmad, Seong Lin Teoh, Jaya Kumar, Mohamad Fairuz Yahaya | | Antioxidants. 2023; 12(2): 427 | | [Pubmed] | [DOI] | | 4 |
Malicia honey (Mimosa quadrivalvis L.) produced by the jandaíra bee (Melipona subnitida D.) improves depressive-like behaviour, somatic, biochemical and inflammatory parameters of obese rats |
|
| Maria Luiza Rolim Bezerra, Mirela Gouveia-Nhanca, Artur D'Angelo da Silva Andrade, Rafael Oliveira Pinheiro, Adriano Francisco Alves, Maria Carolina de Paiva Sousa, Marcos dos Santos Lima, Marciane Magnani, Jailane de Souza Aquino | | Food Research International. 2022; : 112391 | | [Pubmed] | [DOI] | | 5 |
History, phytochemistry, experimental pharmacology and clinical uses of honey: A comprehensive review with special reference to Unani medicine |
|
| Sadia Nikhat, Mohammad Fazil | | Journal of Ethnopharmacology. 2022; 282: 114614 | | [Pubmed] | [DOI] | | 6 |
Behavioural alterations induced by intermittent ethanol intake and noise exposure in adolescent rats |
|
| Gustavo Ezequiel Buján, Luciana D'Alessio, Héctor Alejandro Serra, Sonia Jazmín Molina, Laura Ruth Guelman | | European Journal of Neuroscience. 2022; | | [Pubmed] | [DOI] | | 7 |
Noise-induced hippocampal oxidative imbalance and aminoacidergic neurotransmitters alterations in developing male rats: Influence of enriched environment during adolescence |
|
| Sonia Jazmín Molina, Gustavo Ezequiel Buján, Laura Ruth Guelman | | Developmental Neurobiology. 2021; 81(2): 164 | | [Pubmed] | [DOI] | | 8 |
Effects of early noise exposure on hippocampal-dependent behaviors during adolescence in male rats: influence of different housing conditions |
|
| Sonia Jazmín Molina, Ángel Emanuel Lietti, Candela Sofía Carreira Caro, Gustavo Ezequiel Buján, Laura Ruth Guelman | | Animal Cognition. 2021; | | [Pubmed] | [DOI] | | 9 |
Tualang Honey: A Decade of Neurological Research |
|
| Khairunnuur Fairuz Azman, Che Badariah Abd Aziz, Rahimah Zakaria, Asma Hayati Ahmad, Nazlahshaniza Shafin, Che Aishah Nazariah Ismail | | Molecules. 2021; 26(17): 5424 | | [Pubmed] | [DOI] | | 10 |
Physicochemical and Medicinal Properties of Tualang, Gelam and Kelulut Honeys: A Comprehensive Review |
|
| Datu Agasi Mohd Kamal, Siti Fatimah Ibrahim, Haziq Kamal, Mohd Izhar Ariff Mohd Kashim, Mohd Helmy Mokhtar | | Nutrients. 2021; 13(1): 197 | | [Pubmed] | [DOI] | | 11 |
GELENEKSEL VE TAMAMLAYICI TIPTA APITERAPI VE KULLANIM ALANLARI |
|
| Türkan EKICI, Asuman GÖLGELI | | Saglik Bilimleri Dergisi. 2021; | | [Pubmed] | [DOI] | | 12 |
Simultaneous exposure to noise and carbon monoxide increases the risk of Alzheimer's disease: a literature review |
|
| Fereshteh Bagheri, Vahid Rashedi | | Medical Gas Research. 2020; 10(2): 85 | | [Pubmed] | [DOI] | | 13 |
Amnesia-ameliorative effect of a quinoline derivative through regulation of oxidative/cholinergic systems and Na+/K+-ATPase activity in mice |
|
| Cristiane Luchese, Ane G. Vogt, Mikaela P. Pinz, Angélica S. dos Reis, Carolina B. Gomes, Diego Alves, Ethel A. Wilhelm | | Metabolic Brain Disease. 2020; 35(4): 589 | | [Pubmed] | [DOI] | | 14 |
Honey and obesity-related dysfunctions: a summary on health benefits |
|
| Simona Terzo, Flavia Mulè, Antonella Amato | | The Journal of Nutritional Biochemistry. 2020; 82: 108401 | | [Pubmed] | [DOI] | | 15 |
Natural Products and Their Bioactive Compounds: Neuroprotective Potentials against Neurodegenerative Diseases |
|
| Nur Shafika Mohd Sairazi, K. N. S. Sirajudeen | | Evidence-Based Complementary and Alternative Medicine. 2020; 2020: 1 | | [Pubmed] | [DOI] | | 16 |
Tualang Honey and its Methanolic Fraction Improve LPS-induced Learning and Memory Impairment in Male Rats: Comparison with Memantine |
|
| Wan M.H.W. Yaacob, Idris Long, Rahimah Zakaria, Zahiruddin Othman | | Current Nutrition & Food Science. 2020; 16(3): 333 | | [Pubmed] | [DOI] | | 17 |
Stingless Bee Honey Reduces Anxiety and Improves Memory of the Metabolic Disease-induced Rats |
|
| Nurul ‘Ain Arshad, Teoh Seong Lin, Mohamad Fairuz Yahaya | | CNS & Neurological Disorders - Drug Targets. 2020; 19(2): 115 | | [Pubmed] | [DOI] | |
|
|
 |
 |
|
|
|