ARTICLES |
[Download PDF] |
|
Year : 2003 | Volume
: 6
| Issue : 21 | Page : 77--89 |
Noise and mental performance : Personality attributes and noise sensitivity
G Belojevic, B Jakovljevic, V Slepcevic University of Belgrade, Faculty of Medicine, Institute of Hygiene and Medical Ecology, Serbia
Correspondence Address:
G Belojevic Institute of Hygiene and Medical Ecology School of Medicine, University of Belgrade Dr Subotica 8, 11000 Belgrade, Serbia
Abstract
The contradictory and confusing results in noise research on humans may partly be due to individual differences between the subjects participating in different studies. This review is based on a twelve year research on the role of neuroticism, extroversion and subjective noise sensitivity during mental work in noisy environment. Neurotic persons might show enhanced �DQ�arousability�DQ� i.e. their arousal level increases more in stress. Additional unfavorable factors for neurotics are worrying and anxiety, which might prevent them coping successfully with noise, or some other stressors during mental performance. In numerous experiments introverts have showed higher sensitivity to noise during mental performance compared to extroverts, while extroverts often cope with a boring task even by requesting short periods of noise during performance. Correlation analyses have regularly revealed a highly significant negative relation between extroversion and noise annoyance during mental processing. Numerous studies have shown that people with high noise sensitivity may be prevented from achieving the same work results as other people in noisy environment, thus leading to psycho�somatic, neurotic or other difficulties. Positive relation between noise annoyance and subjective noise sensitivity might be very strong. Our results have shown, after matching with the results of other relevant studies, that more stable personality, with extroversive tendencies and with a relatively lower subjective noise sensitivity measured with standard questionnaires, may be expected to better adapt to noise during mental performance, compared to people with opposite personality traits.
How to cite this article:
Belojevic G, Jakovljevic B, Slepcevic V. Noise and mental performance : Personality attributes and noise sensitivity.Noise Health 2003;6:77-89
|
How to cite this URL:
Belojevic G, Jakovljevic B, Slepcevic V. Noise and mental performance : Personality attributes and noise sensitivity. Noise Health [serial online] 2003 [cited 2023 Dec 5 ];6:77-89
Available from: https://www.noiseandhealth.org/text.asp?2003/6/21/77/31680 |
Full Text
Introduction
According to William James (1890) "the mind, in short, works on the data it receives, very much as a sculptor works on his block of stone. In a sense the statue stood there from eternity. But there are a thousand different ones beside it, and the sculptor alone is to thank for having extricated this one from the rest� Other sculptors, other statues from the same stone! Other minds, other worlds from the same monotonous and inexpressive chaos!"
With the kind of anatomical, physiological and neurological systems Man as a species possesses, he can adapt himself to almost any environment, solve almost any problem. In contrast, man as an individual is limited. This is not necessarily because he lacks any of the potentialities we attribute to the human species, but because he lacks time (Murphy, 1958).
Each individual begins with one particular fraction of the human gene pool, facilitating development in some directions, retarding it in others. Broad social policies, differing from one period to another, help to determine which human potentialities individuals will be encouraged and stimulated to develop. We are witnessing in our time one of these major policy changes. Due to automation and information "boom" in all parts of society, fewer and fewer personalities with superior physical strength and manual skills are needed, but more and more people with superior ability to manipulate symbols i.e. to think.
By the term - "mental performance", various forms of reaction may be considered (e.g. control activity, rapidity of reaction, learning, memorizing or intelligence). While perceptual or motor performance involves primarily muscular activity or sensorial responses, mental or cognitive performance is primarily based on mental activity.
In mental performance there are several stages: 1) sensory processes, 2) perceptual and attentional, 3) short term memory, 4) long term memory, 5) central and specific processors. Out of the huge amount of information detected by sense organs during a cognitive task, some parts are identified, selected and organized through perceptual and attentional processes. In further processing, relevant data may be "filed" and compared with those already kept in long term memory. In tasks with the highest mental load, thinking i. e. central processing of these data, is required in order to select and execute the appropriate response. In all stages there are individually determined capacity limitations which may lead to disturbance by some intrusive factors on processing (Gamberale et al. 1990).
In its nonverbal and verbal forms, the auditory environment is vital for human functioning at both cognitive and emotional levels. In nonverbal noise physical components dominate, while in verbal noise semantic components dominate. On the other hand, auditory stimulation can become a highly destructive source of negative experiences. Auditory stimulation may include physical and semantic components that cause frustrating, irritation and inefficiency. They may become a serious threat to our wellbeing and health. Due to intensive industrialization and urbanization followed by traffic "boom", noise or "unwanted sound" (Burns 1973) has become a ubiquitous stressful factor, both in living and in occupational environments. The public sees this trend as a serious threat to human well being, so that in opinion polls noise has regularly been ranked among the most annoying single environmental problems.
In the occupational environment, noise is a major noxious factor. In highly industrialized countries like Sweden about 40% of the total work force consider themselves to be exposed to noise. For males it is 68%, and even for white� collar workers it is about 25%. However, physical noise measures are usually found to explain only 14%-25% of the variance in workers annoyance (Kjellberg et al 1988, Kjellberg 1990).
Due to noise abatement policies, the number of workplaces with high noise levels is expected to decrease in most countries. Noise levels at 90 dB and above are of limited practical interest in performance studies, as these levels should be avoided in working milieu for reasons of hearing protection.
However, at many previously quiet workplaces the new sources of moderately loud noise have been introduced, namely various automates, instruments, computers, printers, ventilation systems etc. For instance, in a modern operating theatre, which is generally regarded as quiet (Leq about 50 dBA), Hodge and Thompson (1990) have found 27 sources of sound, with peak levels ranging from 52 to 108 dB. This revolution in automation has given priority to manipulating symbols instead of tools i.e. to mental over physical work. This is why it may be assumed that mental performance in moderately loud noise might be more and more frequent problem in occupational hygiene. Although Broadbent (1957) concluded that no detrimental effect of noise on performance should be expected at levels lower than 90 dB, contemporary research tends to doubt this claim. Depending on the complexity and nature of a task, the experimental setting and the choice of subjects, noise levels sufficient for performance intrusion could be lowered to 51 dB(A) (Kjellberg and Wide, 1988).
Noise is a highly relative construct. There is a common denominator, however: unwanted sound. Noise induced stress means that the organism is unable to ignore, block out or otherwise cope with the unwanted sound, or that it is able to do so only at a cost. There are two major, relatively independent target areas for noise research:
Noise affects the person who is "doing nothing" by annoying him.
Noise affects the person who works by impairing his performance or making it more costly in terms of increased effort.
Against this background, studies on the importance of noise for work performance have a high priority in occupational medicine. The aspects of main interest in the studies of the effects of the work environment on performance may be: productivity effects, safety risks or mental and neurological functioning. The objects of main interest may be identification of sensitive individuals or identification of environments that lower productivity or have high accident risk or are hazardous to health and well being. Practical individual problems have provided the impetus for such studies and their principal aim has been to improve productivity and the quality of job performance, by providing a suitable physical work environment. Performance measurement may also be used for predicting accident risk or as an indicator of the state of various CNS functions. When safety risks are to be assessed, it is necessary to observe performance in the real work task or a simulation of that task. If on the other hand, the aim is to assess mental or neurological functioning, the realism of the task is of little consequence (Gamberale et al 1990).
Studies of Mental Performance
The contradictory and confusing results in noise research may partly be due to individual differences between the subjects participating in different studies. It is a well-known everyday experience that people react to noise in different ways. Some are seriously disturbed, others are not affected at all and still others might even improve their performance. Such opposing trends for subgroups mask each other and perhaps lead to the incorrect conclusion that noise does not affect performance (Auble and Britton 1958).
With respect to general intelligence, it is assumed that less able subjects would be more susceptible to harmful effects of noise, because they probably are more dependent on task relevant information than the more able subjects. Furthermore, when doing mental work in noise, they probably have less resources to call upon, while the more able subjects can exploit their resources if the need arises.
Studies on people living in areas with a high level of aircraft-noise showed that their everyday errors (failures of attention, memory and action) appear more frequently, compared to people living in low-noise areas (Smith and Stansfeld 1986). Children in noise-impacted areas have been shown to display impaired mental performance, including incidental learning and visual search (Heft 1979), reading achievement (Green et al. 1982), and puzzle solving (Cohen and Weinstein 1981). Self-reports of everyday errors (failures of attention, memory and action), given by people in an area with high aircraft noise and those of a similar group who lived in an area with a low level of noise, the high noise group reported a greater frequency of everyday errors (Smith and Stansfeld, 1986). An interview cross-sectional study carried out on 413 residents in the centre of Belgrade showed the significant behavioral effects of road traffic noise (Leq>65 dB (A)) in terms of more frequent intention to change the place of living, shortening the daily period of open windows and worse interpersonal relationship between dwellers (Belojevic et al. 1997).
In the occupational environment, noise impairs tasks involving a high mental load and high control precision, has no effect on manual dexterity and even facilitates tasks involving physical strength (Levy-Leboyer and Moser, 1988).
One of the earliest field studies on effects of noise on performance showed that, over the course of one year, weavers (10) with normal hearing, who wore earplugs in 96 dBA industrial noise, had better loom output (quantity and quality of the cloth) than those who had no hearing protection (Weston and Adams 1935). It has been reported that workers in textile mill departments with high noise levels (above 90 dBA) showed slightly lower productivity (1.24%, p65 dB (A). Multiple regression analysis revealed that neuroticism was the best individual predictor of subjective noise sensitivity, for both sexes in the noisy area and for women only, in the quiet area. Age, education level and introversion were not significantly related to noise sensitivity. Positive relation between reported noise annoyance and noise sensitivity was highly significant (Belojevic and Jakovljevic 2001).
Various criteria have been used to explain individual differences in noise sensitivity. One of them is the auditory acuity, but it seems that there is no significant relation between auditory thresholds as measured by audiometer and subjective noise sensitivity, although necessarily severe hearing impairment will reduce annoyance reactions to noise (Stansfeld et al. 1985). This favors the assumption that subjective noise sensitivity is a function of cognitive process rather then merely a function of a peripheral auditory acuity.
Subjective noise sensitivity and noise annoyance were shown to be related to complaint behavior (Weinstein 1978), but the criticism toward the environmental factors, including noise might be influenced by other subjective variables besides noise sensitivity (Levy-Leboyer et al. 1976). Therefore, complaint behavior and subjective noise sensitivity are not directly related (Cohen and Weinstein 1981). Noise sensitivity, as a subjective variable, is likely to be randomly distributed with respect to age, sex, social class, education and job responsibility (Moreira and Bryan 1972, Taylor 1984). It also seems that differences in sensitivity to noise are not confined to noise nuisance only, but could be extrapolated to other environmental nuisances (Langdon 1976), as noise sensitive persons tend to express higher criticism toward their environment in general (Weinstein 1982). The correlation between noise-sensitivity scale and the personality tests revealed that sensitivity to noise clearly has much in common with a desire for privacy and that the tendency to be bothered by noise is related to a tendency to express annoyance about a wide variety of nuisances. Noise sensitive subjects were shown to be generally lower in dominance, capacity for status, sociability and social presence. In investigating the personal attributes of highly noise sensitive persons it was shown that they have significantly less intellectual ability, fewer social skills, and a stronger desire for privacy than those who are not sensitive. Noise sensitive individuals are more likely to be critical of their surroundings and to express annoyance about situations generally recognized as being irritating (Weinstein 1978). It is questioned whether the modest link between introversion and noise sensitivity is due to direct arousing effects of noise on the central nervous system, or to the fact that noise frequently has interpersonal significance and is seen as an intrusion by those who are ill at ease in social settings and prize privacy (psychosocial effect). Weinstein's Noise sensitivity Scale was applied in 215 Swedish students (77% female). Mean NSS score was 57,54 � 12.61 (0-5). All 21 items were in negative correlation to extroversion and 17 statistically significant (p<0.10). The relation between total NSS score and extroversion was highly significant (p<0.001). There was a marginally significant positive correlation between noise sensitivity and neuroticism, but it disappeared when controlling for extroversion. (Dornic and Ekehammar, 1990).
An important question is whether there may be a relationship between noise sensitivity and other traits or personality characteristics that would make it possible to predict the individual's responses to noise in as many situations as possible. On the whole, research into this problem has brought about few unambiguous results. Most of the data reported up to now are inconclusive and often conflicting. The number of studies in which a clear relationship between noise sensitivity and some other relevant variable has been found is small. For instance, Weinstein (1978) found self-reported noise sensitivity to be associated with intelligence, self-confidence in social interactions and need for privacy. Noise annoyance scales have been used as a measure of neuroticism, but Broadbent's selective review (1971) points out inconclusive and controversial nature of such a relationship.
Iwata (1984) reported that individuals whose level of noise sensitivity (as measured by a scale) was high, tended to be less healthy than those displaying low level of noise sensitivity. Iwata also found that noise-sensitive people were those who could be identified as maladjusted personalities. On the other hand, no simple or obvious relationship was found between sensitivity to noise (in terms of annoyance) on the one hand, and age, sex, education, income and job responsibility, on the other. This is true both of community noise surveys (Weinstein, 1978), and of a laboratory study (Moreira & Bryan, 1972). In the latter study, the above listed variables did not appear to be related to personality traits as determined by the EPI and the MMPI, but were related to some personality measures derived from the Rorschach test.
Jelinkova et al (1988) showed that noise sensitive people had a reduced work ability and attention when exposed to recorded traffic noise at a level of 75 dB (A) Leq, compared to persons tolerant to noise. In another field study, in which noise sensitivity was linked to complaints about chronic exposure to environmental noise, complainers were shown to perform worse on a vigilance task than non-complainers (Poenary et al. 1987). Although Dornic et al. (1989) failed to confirm that subjective noise sensitivity correlated with performance on vigilance, information processing, reasoning and incidental learning tasks, they found that irritation by noise and general annoyance during performance were significantly higher in noise-sensitive persons. This relation between noise annoyance and subjective noise sensitivity has been shown to be very strong (Ohrstrom et al. 1988), presenting a possible clue in the explanations of the individual differences in performance in noise. An experimental study on students performing four cognitive tasks under recorded traffic noise (Leq = 55 dB (A) and 75 dB (A)) showed that subjective noise sensitivity was the primary factor responsible for significant differences in mental performance in a short-term memory and a mental arithmetic task (Belojevic et al 1992). Although annoyance has generally been regarded as a dependent variable in noise studies, it might act as an intermediate factor as well (Gulian 1973), as annoyance is likely to have a feedback on the psycho-physiological state and thus on performance, just as performance level has a certain influence on annoyance level and arousal.
Noise annoyance as a negative attitude towards a specific noise or noisy environments, is a feeling of displeasure associated with actual noise, which may have a general additive effect, enhancing arousal, distraction and masking during mental performance. Furthermore, it might force the performer to work faster even at the cost of less accuracy, in order to get rid of the unpleasant situation as soon as possible. Annoyance may as well absorb a part of processing capacity that will then not be available for the task, or for coping with intrusion by noise (Dornic 1977, Eysenck 1982). When considered at all, annoyance has typically been dealt with as a dependent variable parallel to performance, rather than a possible mechanism underlying or mediating the effect of noise on information processing (i.e. interme�diate variable). Noise-induced annoyance is likely to affect processing efficiency in several ways. It may further increase arousal triggered by noise and enhance the effects of distraction and masking. Apart from these general, additive effects, annoyance is likely to have an effect of its own. For instance, it may absorb a part of processing capacity that will then not be available for the task (Dornic 1977, Eysenck 1982). Second, annoyance may prompt the performer to work faster, even at the cost of adapting a more risky decision criterion, in order to get rid of the aversive situation as soon as possible.
In summary, noise may prevent people with high noise sensitivity from achieving the same work results as other people, thus leading to psycho�somatic, neurotic or other difficulties. In that case, long-term noise abatement policies should be aimed to adapt work places to more sensitive workers. Noise sensitivity appears to be a complex, multidimensional personal characteristic, possibly stable, but subject to situationally determined (and therefore considerably variable) cognitive and affective factors involving meaning, attitudes, motivation and so on. It is also clear that different criteria, such as annoyance and performance, need not necessarily yield an identical picture of an individual's noise tolerance.
Conclusion
Mental performance in noise is influenced by a network of interrelated factors classified in three major groups: characteristics of a task, environment and personality. In a twelve year research period we have concentrated on individual differences in adaptation to noise during mental work, namely on the role of neuroticism, extroversion and subjective noise sensitivity. Our results have shown, after matching with the results of other relevant studies, that more stable personality, with extroversive tendencies and with a relatively lower subjective noise sensitivity measured with standard questionnaires, may be expected to better adapt to noise during mental performance, compared to people with opposite personality traits.
Acknowledgments
This review is a result of the the following three consecutive projects:
Effects of noise on work performance with regard to subjective noise sensitivity, 1990-1991, Department of Environmetal Medicine, University of Gothenburg (financially supported by the Swedish Work Environment Fund). The effects of road traffic noise on mental health of an urban population, 1993-1996, Institute or Hygiene and Medical Ecology, Faculty of Medicine, University of Belgrade (Ministry of Science and Technology, Republic of Serbia). Psyhophysiological effects of noise, 1996-2000, Institute or Hygiene and Medical Ecology, Faculty of Medicine, University of Belgrade, (Ministry of Science and Technology, Republic of Serbia).
Goran Belojevic was the Lead Investigator in these projects.
The authors are thankful to Swedish colleagues from the Department of Environmental Medicine, University of Gothenburg:
Prof Evy Ohrstrom for substantial role in designing and performing the experiments as well as in writing investigation report and a scientific paper,
Prof Ragnar Rylander for support, supervision and advices during the investigation,
Mr. Martin Bjorkman, PhD, Civil Engineer, for technical support in designing and performing human experiments and Ms. Ann Brit Skanberg, for assisting in research data input.[85]
References
1 | Anderson CMB (1971) The measurement of attitude to noise and noises. National Physical Laboratory Acoustics Report, Ac 52. |
2 | Auble D, Britton N. (1958) Anxiety as a factor influencing routine performance under auditory stimuli. J General Psychol, 58: 111-114. |
3 | Baker MA, Holding DH, Loeb M (1984) Noise, sex and time of day effects in a mathematics task. Ergonomics 27: 67 - 80. |
4 | Belojevic G, Ohrstrom E, Rylander R. (1992) Effects of noise on mental performance with regard to subjective noise sensitivity. Int Arch Occup Environ Health 64 (4): 293-301. |
5 | Belojevic G, Jakovljevic B, Aleksic O. (1997) Subjective reactions to traffic noise with regard to some personality traits. Environ Int 23 (2): 221-226. |
6 | Belojevic G, Jakovljevic B. (2001) Factors influencing subjective noise sensitivity in an urban population. Noise & Health 4 (13): 17-24. |
7 | Belojevic G, Slepcevic V, Jakovljevic B (2001) Mental performance in noise: The role of introversion. J Environ Psychol 21 (2) : 209 - 213. |
8 | Bloom BS. (1964) Stability and change in human characteristics. New York: Wiley. |
9 | Bregman HL and Pearson RG (1972) Development of a noise annoyance sensitivity scale. NASA CR-1954, Springfield, Virginia: National Technical Information Service. |
10 | Broadbent DE. (1957) Effects of noise on behavior. In: Harris CM (Ed.): Handbook of noise control, New York: Mc Graw-Hill, pp. 10-1-10-34. |
11 | Broadbent DE, Little EAJ. (1960) Effects of noise reduction in a work situation. Occup Psychol, 34:133-140. |
12 | Broadbent DE. (1961) The twenty dials and twenty lights test under noise conditions. Rept. 160/51, Medical Research Council, Cambridge, England. |
13 | Broadbent DE (1971) Decision and stress, London: Academic Press. |
14 | Burns W (1973) Noise and man. London: John Murray, p. 5. |
15 | Campbell, J. B. and Hawley, C. W. (1982). Study habits and Eysenck's theory of extroversion-introversion. Journal of Research in Personality, 16, 139-146. |
16 | Cohen S and Weinstein N (1981) Nonauditory effects of noise on behavior and health. J Soc Issues 37:36. |
17 | Cohen A, Hummel WF, Turner JN, Durer-Dobos FN (1966) Effects of noise on task performance. Rept. RR-4. US Department of health, education and welfare. Occupational Health Research and Training Facility, Cincinnati, Ohio, USA. |
18 | Daee S, Wilding JM (1977) Effects of high intensity white noise on short-term memory for position in a list and sequence. Br J Psychol 68: 335- 349. |
19 | Daoussis I and Mc Kelvie S. (1986). Musical preferences and effects of music on a reading comprehension test for extraverts and introverts. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 62, 283-289. |
20 | Davies ADM, Davies DR (1975) The effects of noise and time of day upon age differences in performance at two checking tasks. Ergonomics 18: 321 - 336. |
21 | Davies DR, Hockey GRJ, Taylor A (1969) Varied auditory stimulation, temperament differences and vigilance performance. Br J Psychol 60: 453 - 457. |
22 | Dornic S (1975) Some studies on the retention of order information. In: Rabbitt P, Dornic s (eds.) Attention and performance. London: Academic Press. |
23 | Dornic S (1977) Mental load, effort and individual differences. Reports from the Department of Psychology, University of Stockholm, No 509. |
24 | Dornic S, Laaksonen T, Ekehammar B. (1989) Noise sensitivity: general self report vs. noise effect in laboratory situations. Technical Reports from the project �Human information processing under stress�, Department of Psychology, University of Stockholm. |
25 | Dornic S. (1990) Noise and information processing: finding, trends and issues. Reports from the Department of Psychology, Stockholm University, No. 715. |
26 | Dornic S. and Ekehammar B. (1990) Extroversion, neuroticism and noise sensitivity. Person Individ Diff 11 (9): 989-992. |
27 | Ekehammar B and Dornic S (1990) Weinstein's noise sensitivity scale: reliability and construct validity. Percept Mot Skills, 70: 129-130. |
28 | Eysenck HJ (1947) Dimensions of personality. London: Routledge and Kegan Paul. |
29 | Eysenck, HJ. (1967). The biological basis of personality. Springfield, IL: Thomas. |
30 | Eysenck HJ (1970) Personality and tolerance for noise. IN: Taylor W (ed.) Proceeding of the Symposium on Psychological Effects of Noise. Dept Social and Occupational Medicine, University of Dundee. |
31 | Eysenck MW (1977) Human memory: Theory, research and individual differences. Oxford: Pergamon. |
32 | Eysenck MW (1982) Attention and arousal: Cognition and performance. Berlin: Springer. |
33 | Furnham, A., Gunter, B. and Peterson, E. (1994). Televison distraction and the performance of introverts and extroverts. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 8, 705-711. |
34 | Gamberale F, Kjellberg A, Akerstedt T, Johansson G. (1990) Behavioural and psychophysiological effects of the physical work environment. Scand J Work Environ Health 16 (suppl 1): 5-16. |
35 | Geen RG. (1984) Preferred stimulation levels in introverts and extroverts: Effects on arousal and performance. J Personal Soc Psychol 46:1303-1312. |
36 | Griffiths ED and Delauzun FR (1977) Individual differences in sensitivity to traffic noise; an empirical study. J Sound Vib 55:93 - 107. |
37 | Goldstein J and DeJoy DM (1980) Behavioral and performance effects of noise: Perspectives for research. In: Tobias JV, Jansen G, Ward WD (eds.) Proceedings of the International Congress on Noise as a Public Health Problem, Rockville MD: ASHA Reports 10, pp. 369 -374. |
38 | Gray, J. (1964). Strength of the nervous system and levels of arousal: A reinterpretation. In J. Gray (Ed), Pavlov's typology (pp. 289-366). Oxford: Pergamon. |
39 | Gray, J. (1981). A critique of Eysenck's theory of personality. In H. Eysenck (Ed.), a model for personality. Berlin: Springer-Verlag. |
40 | Green KB, Pasternack BS, Shore RE (1982) Effects of aircraft noise on reading ability of school age children. Arch Environ Health 37: 24 - 31. |
41 | Gulian E (1973) Psychological consequences of exposure to noise, facts and explanations. In: Ward WD (ed.) |
42 | Proceedings of the 2nd International Congress on Noise as a Public Health Problem, Dubrovnik, Washington DC: EPA, pp. 379 -387. |
43 | Heft H (1979) Background and focal environmental conditions of the home and attention in young children. J Appl Soc Psychol 9: 47 - 69. |
44 | Hockey GRJ (1970) Signal probability and spatial location as possible bases for increased selectivity in noise. Quart J Exp Psychol 22: 37- 42. |
45 | Hockey GRJ (1972) Effects of noise on human efficiency and some individual differences. J Sound Vib 20 (3): 299 - 304. |
46 | Hodge B. and Thompson JF. (1990) Noise pollution in the operating theatre. Lancet 335: 891-894. |
47 | Iwata O ( 1984) The relationship of noise sensitivity to health and personality. Jap Psychol Res 26: 75 - 81. |
48 | James W (1890) Principles of Psychology. New York: Holt, Vol I, pp. 288-289. |
49 | Jelinkova Z, Picek M, Hyncica V (1989) Psychophysiological aspects of noise influence. Int J Psychophysiol 7: 249 - 250. |
50 | Kirk RE, Hecht E. (1963) Maintenance of vigilance by programmed noise. Percept Mot Skill 16: 553-560. |
51 | Kjellberg A, Landstrom U, Lofstedt P, Wide P, Akerlund E. (1988) Evaluation of noise and annoyance in working environments: a methodological study. Solna (Sweden): National Institute of Occupational Health, p. 30 |
52 | Kjellberg A (1990a) Noise annoyance during the performance of different non-auditory tasks. Proceedings of "Inter-Noise 90", Goteborg, vol. 2, pp. 1167 - 1170. |
53 | Kjellberg A (1990b) Subjective, behavioral and psychophysiological effects of noise. Scand J Work Environ Health 16 (Suppl.): 29-38. |
54 | Kjellberg A. Wide P. (1988) Effects of simulated ventilation noise on performance of a grammatical reasoning task. In; Berglund B, Berglund U, Karlsson J, |
55 | Lindvall T, ed. Proceedings 5t h International Congress Noise as a Public Health Problem; Vol. 3, Stockholm, Byggforskningskadet, pp. 31-36. |
56 | Kovrigin SD and Mikheyev AP (1965) The effect of noise level on working efficiency. Rept N65-28297, Joint Publication Research Service, Washington, D.C. |
57 | Kryter KD and Poza F (1980) Autonomic system activity and performance on a psychomotor task in noise. J Acoust Soc Am 67: 2096 - 2099. |
58 | Langdon FJ (1976) Noise nuisance caused by road traffic in residential areas: Part III. J Sound Vib 49: 241 - 256. |
59 | Levy - Leboyer C, Vedrenne B, Veyssiere M (1976) Psychologie differentielle des genes dues au bruit. Annee Psychologique 76: 245 -256. |
60 | Levy-Leboyer A. and Moser G. (1988) Noise effects on two industrial tasks. In: Berglund B, Berglund U, Karlsson J, Lindvall T (Eds.) Proceedings of the Fifth International Congress on Noise as a Public Health Problem, Swedish Council for Building research, Stockholm, pp. 43-48. |
61 | Loeb M (1981) The present state of research on the effects of noise: Are we asking the right questions? J Aud Res 21: 93 - 104. |
62 | O'Malley J and Poplawsky A (1971) Noise induced arousal and breadth of attention. Percept Motor Skills, 33: 887 - 890. |
63 | Mandler G, Savason SB (1952) A study of anxiety and learning. J Abnorm Soc Psychol 47: 166-173. |
64 | Mc Kennel AC and Hunt EA (1966) Noise annoyance in central London. The Government Social Survey, London: HMSO. |
65 | Morgenstern, S., Hodgson, R. J. & Law, L. (1974). Work efficiency and personality. Ergonomics, 17, 211-220. |
66 | Moreira NM, Bryan ME. (1972) Noise annoyance susceptibility. J Sound Vib 21 (4) 449 - 462. |
67 | Morgenstern FS, Hodgson RJ, Law L (1974) Work efficiency and personality: A comparison of introverted and extraverted subjects exposed to conditions of distraction and distortion of stimulus in a learning task. Ergonomics 17: 211 - 220. |
68 | Murphy G (1958) Human potentialities. New York: Basic Books. |
69 | Noweir MH (1984) Noise exposure as related to productivity, disciplinary actions, absenteeism and accidents among textile workers. J Safety Res, 15: 163�174. |
70 | Nurmi J and von Wright J (1983) Interactive effect of noise, neuroticism and state of anxiety in the learning and recall of a textbook passage. Human Learning 2: 119 - 125. |
71 | Ohrstom E, Bjorkman M, Rylander R (1988) Noise annoyance with regard to neuro-physiological sensitivity, subjective noise sensitivity and personality variables. Psychol Med 18: 605 - 613. |
72 | Poenary S, Rouhani S, Poggi D, Moch A, Colas C, Cohen E, Blacker C, Belon JP, Gauge P, Dall'Ava-Santucci (1987) Study of the pathophysiological effects of chronic exposure to environmental noise in man. J Acoust Letters 11 (5) : 80 -87. |
73 | Shanmugan TE and Santhanam MC (1964) Personality differences in serial learning when interference is presented at the marginal visual level. J Ind Acad Appl Psychol 1: 25-28. |
74 | Smith AP (1968) Acute effects of noise exposure. An experimental investigation of the effects of noise and task parameters on cognitive vigilance task. Int Arch Occup Environ Health 60: 307 -310. |
75 | Smith AP, Stansfeld S. (1986) Aircraft noise exposure, noise sensitivity and everyday errors. Environ Behav, 18: 214-226. |
76 | Stansfeld SA, Clark CR, Turpion G, Jenkins LM, Tarnopolsky A (1985) Sensitivity to noise in community sample: II measurement of psychophysiological indices. Psychol Med 15: 255 - 263. |
77 | Stansfeld SA (1988) Sensitivity to noise - source of error or stable personal characteristic. In: Rossi G (ed.) Proceedings of the International Congress on Noise as a Public Health Problem, Milano: Centro Ricerche e Studi Amplifon, pp. 57 - 64. |
78 | Stelmach, R. (1981). The psychophysiology of extroversion and neuroticism. In H. Eysenck (Ed.), A model for personality. Berlin: Springer-Verlag. |
79 | Taylor SM (1984) A path model of aircraft noise annoyance. J Sound Vib 96: 243 - 260. |
80 | Tyler LE (1965) Individual differences in intelligence. In: Tyler LE. The psychology of human differences. New York: Appleton Century Crofts (Meredith Publishing Co.), pp. 61-99. |
81 | Vermonlayeva-Tomina, L. B. (1964). In J. Gray (Ed.), Pavlov's typology. Oxford: Pergamon. |
82 | von Wright J, Vauras M. (1980) Interactive effects of noise and neuroticism on recall from semantic memory. Scand J Psychol 21: 97-101. |
83 | Weinstein ND (1978) Individual differences in reactions to noise : A longitudinal study in a college dormitory. J Appl Psychol 63 (4): 458-466. |
84 | Weinstein ND (1982) Community noise problems: evidence against adaptation. J Env Psychol 2:87-97. |
85 | Weston HC, Adams S (1935) The performance of weavers under varying conditions of noise. Indust Res Board, London, England, report No. 70. |
|