ARTICLE |
[Download PDF] |
|
Year : 2014 | Volume
: 16
| Issue : 68 | Page : 47--56 |
Noise sensitivity and diminished health: Testing moderators and mediators of the relationship
Erin M Hill, Rex Billington, Chris Krägeloh Department of Psychology, Auckland University of Technology, Auckland, New Zealand
Correspondence Address:
Erin M Hill Department of Psychology, West Chester University of Pennsylvania, Pennsylvania 19383, United States
Abstract
The concept of noise sensitivity emerged in public health and psychoacoustic research to help explain individual differences in reactions to noise. Noise sensitivity has been associated with health problems, but the mechanisms underlying this relationship have yet to be fully examined. Participants (n = 1102) were residents of Auckland, New Zealand, who completed questionnaires and returned them through the post. Models of noise sensitivity and health were tested in the analyses using bootstrapping methods to examine indirect effects. Results indicated that gender and noise exposure were not significant moderators in the model. Perceived stress and sleep problems were significant mediators of the relationship between noise sensitivity and subjective health complaints, even after controlling for the influence of neuroticism. However, the relationship between noise sensitivity and mental health complaints (anxiety and depression) was accounted for by the variance explained by neuroticism. Overall, this study provides considerable understanding of the relationship between noise sensitivity and health problems and identifies areas for further research in the field.
How to cite this article:
Hill EM, Billington R, Krägeloh C. Noise sensitivity and diminished health: Testing moderators and mediators of the relationship.Noise Health 2014;16:47-56
|
How to cite this URL:
Hill EM, Billington R, Krägeloh C. Noise sensitivity and diminished health: Testing moderators and mediators of the relationship. Noise Health [serial online] 2014 [cited 2023 Dec 9 ];16:47-56
Available from: https://www.noiseandhealth.org/text.asp?2014/16/68/47/127855 |
Full Text
Introduction
Researchers interested in the impact of environmental noise on health and well-being have long acknowledged the important role that noise sensitivity plays in the relationship. [1],[2],[3],[4],[5] Noise sensitivity is a strong predictor of noise annoyance [4],[6],[7],[8] and has been found to moderate the association between noise exposure and self-reported health. [9] Although noise sensitivity is seldom the focus of public health research investigations, there is mounting evidence to suggest that noise sensitivity may be a health risk factor unto itself. [10],[11],[12],[13],[14]
Noise sensitivity has been described as a complex and multifaceted construct that encompasses physiological reactions to noise, psychological reactivity and coping mechanisms. [15] Although noise sensitivity is often included as a potential moderator or covariate in acoustics-based and public health research on the effects of environmental noise, [8],[16],[17] there is limited evidence to suggest that noise sensitivity involves a sensory component. [18],[19] Noise sensitive individuals do not have superior hearing abilities, as assessed by a variety of psychoacoustic outcomes including absolute hearing thresholds, intensity discrimination and auditory reaction time. [18],[20],[21]
Instead of involving a sensory component, noise sensitivity appears instead to be related to attitudinal perceptions; negative correlations between noise sensitivity and the uncomfortable loudness level and discomfort thresholds have been reported. [3],[22] As such, noise sensitivity has been labeled a personality trait that influences an individual's reaction to noise. [15],[18],[23]
A number of studies have revealed a positive relationship between noise sensitivity and another well-recognized personality trait, neuroticism, [3],[11],[24],[25] defined as an individual's tendency to experience negative emotions including anxiety, anger and depression. [26] Notably, Smith et al. [27] suggested that the relationship between noise sensitivity and self-reported health could be accounted for by negative affectivity (included in their models as a covariate), which they measured using a neuroticism scale. Beyond their study, however, there has been limited research conducted to thoroughly examine the relationship among noise sensitivity, neuroticism and health and well-being. Nonetheless, due to the relationship between neuroticism and noise sensitivity, [11],[24],[25] neuroticism is an important trait to measure and control for in studies assessing the relationship between noise sensitivity and health.
Elevated stress levels may play a key role in better understanding the relationship between noise sensitivity and diminished health. There is evidence to suggest that noise sensitive individuals have a lower threshold for physiological stress reactivity [28],[29],[30] in addition to having a greater emotional response to stressors. [13],[29] Chronic activation of the stress response has been implicated in the development of both physical [31],[32] and mental illness [33] and therefore one possible explanation for the association between noise sensitivity and depreciated well-being is the bodily wear and tear associated with chronic stress. [34]
In addition, sleep disturbance, a well-established risk factor for a range of health problems, [35] may be involved in the relationship between noise sensitivity and health. Kishikawa et al. [9] reported that noise sensitive individuals, particularly those exposed to high levels of environmental noise, were more likely to report somatic complaints, anxiety and insomnia. The authors concluded that the association between noise sensitivity and health complaints is likely explained by sleep disturbance. However, to the best of our knowledge, no studies to date have addressed this research question.
Noise sensitivity has been linked to sleep disturbance in both laboratory-based [36],[37],[38] and community-based studies. [13],[39],[40],[41],[42] In a laboratory study by Marks and Griefahn (2007), noise sensitivity was found to be associated with decreased sleep quality as evidenced by increased body movements throughout the night and difficulty falling asleep after being awoken. Similarly, noise sensitivity has also been linked to sleep disturbance in various community-based studies on road traffic noise, [13],[38],[40],[43] railway noise [39] and aircraft noise. [42] Further, noise sensitivity has also been linked to the use of sleep medication and other psychotropic mediations. [11]
There is some evidence that gender may influence the association between noise sensitivity and health. [12],[13],[44] Nivison and Endresen [13] reported a significant association between noise sensitivity and a variety of health complaints (muscle, intestinal, nervous, cold and flu, heart-related) among females, while, among males, noise sensitivity was only marginally associated with long-term (i.e., >3 years) allergy complaints. Similarly, Babisch [44] found that an association between self-reported doctor-diagnosed diseases was positively associated with noise sensitivity among women, but not men. It appears that the relationship between noise sensitivity and health problems is stronger among women than men. However, the mechanisms by which gender impacts the relationship - be it psychosocial or biological - have not yet been fully examined and gender remains an important consideration in assessing the relationship between noise sensitivity and health problems.
The Present Study
Noise sensitivity appears to be a health risk factor affecting the development of mental and physical health problems [9],[10],[11],[14],[45] and yet the mechanisms underlying the relationship have not yet been fully examined. Therefore, our study tested a model of noise sensitivity and diminished health to evaluate potential moderators and mediators of the relationship [Figure 1]. This model was developed through identification of potential mediators and moderators of the noise sensitivity-health relationship found in the literature.{Figure 1}
In developing the model, stress and sleep problems have been identified as factors that can impact health and well-being [46],[47] and thus were tested as potential mediators in the overall model. Specifically, the experience of perceived stress evokes physiological changes (i.e. release of stress hormones and catecholamines), which, if sustained, can contribute to the development of health problems and the development of disease. [48] Similarly, sleep problems and lack of sleep can impact an individual's health and potentially put an individual at risk for the development of chronic diseases. [49],[50],[51]
The psychological and health effects of noise sensitivity may differ between males and females; therefore, gender was tested as a moderator in the overall model. Finally, noise exposure is an important variable to consider in examining the relationship between noise sensitivity and well-being. An interaction between noise sensitivity and noise exposure is theoretically plausible; individuals who are noise sensitive and live in noisy areas should, theoretically, be more affected by the noise compared to noise sensitive individuals living in quiet areas. Indeed, an interaction between noise exposure and noise sensitivity interaction has been reported in the literature previously, [9] but there has been mixed evidence as well. [10],[11],[45] In order to test the interaction between noise exposure and noise sensitivity, noise exposure was included as a moderator in our model.
Subjective health complaints and mental health complaints (anxiety and depression) were tested in the models as outcome variables. Measures of subjective health complaints have been used previously in investigations on noise exposure, noise sensitivity and health. [13],[17] In addition, mental health problems have been strongly associated with noise sensitivity. [9],[19] Therefore, it was important to consider general subjective health complaints, but mental health problems as well.
Methods
Participants and procedure
Participants were residents of Auckland, New Zealand. Questionnaires were delivered from April to August 2011 (autumn and winter months) to both noise-exposed households (situated near high volume traffic roads) and households in control areas (not situated near high volume traffic roads). Traffic volumes, obtained from Auckland City Council records (collected during 2009), were used a proxy for traffic noise exposure in the present study.
Prior to distribution, questionnaires were coded according to road traffic volume (vehicles/24 h): (1) <10,000, (2) 10,000-20,000, (3) 20,000-30,000 and (4) >30,000. All houses included in the noise-exposed conditions (>10,000 vehicles/24 h) had at least one side of the house within 20m of the road, as determined with the use of Google maps. The delivery of questionnaires to noise-exposed households was exhaustive in that the majority of houses in residential areas on major arterial roadways identified from Auckland City Council data received a questionnaire. Control households were selected with the aid of the Auckland City Council records (traffic volumes < 10,000 vehicles/24 h). Individuals living at the residence, who were over the age of 18, were invited to participate. A total of 1102 residents participated in the study out of 7500 delivered questionnaires (response rate: 14.7%). The study was approved by the authors' university ethics committee.
Measures
Noise sensitivity
A three-item Noise-sensitivity scale (3-NS; [52] ) was used in the present study. The 3-NS involves three statements to which participants respond on Likert scales ranging from (1) completely agree to (5) completely disagree. This format was modified from the original 4-point Likert scale used in the original study by Amann et al. [52] Scores are summed, with elevated scores indicating greater noise sensitivity. Good internal consistency and validity were reported in the research by Amann et al. [52] In this study, internal consistency was acceptable (Cronbach's α =0.68).
Subjective health complaints
The subjective health complaints inventory (SHCI) is a 29-item scale that measures the intensity and frequency of common health complaints in the last 30 days. [53],[54] The questionnaire collects information regarding musculoskeletal pain, pseudo-neurological complaints (e.g., tiredness, dizziness, anxiety and sadness/depression), gastrointestinal problems, allergy complaints and flu complaints.
For each health complaint item, participants rate the extent to which they were suffering from the condition (severity rating) on a 4-point Likert scale ranging from (0) not at all to (3) seriously and they also indicate the number of days they were suffering from the health problem in the past month. A composite score for the scale was computed by adding the total severity ratings for 28 of the items. The sleep problems item was not included in the composite score as it was treated as a mediator variable in the analyses. The SHCI questionnaire has also been used in a large population studies and has been reported to have adequate validity and reliability. [53],[55] In the present study, the overall scale had very good internal consistency (Cronbach's α =0.83).
Anxiety and depression
Information on anxiety and depression was collected through two items on the SHCI. Participants rated the extent to which they had been suffering from anxiety and depression in the past 30 days on a Likert scale ranging from (0) not at all to (3) seriously.
Perceived stress
The perceived stress scale (PSS) is a 10-item self-report measure that was designed for use among community samples with at least intermediate school (to 12 years of age) level education. [56] It measures the extent to which participants consider their lives to have been unpredictable, unmanageable and generally stressful within the past month. Participants respond to each question on a 5-point Likert scale ranging from (0) never to (4) very often. The scale is psychometrically sound, exhibiting adequate reliability and validity. [56] In the present study, the PSS had very good internal consistency (Cronbach's α =0.87).
Sleep problems
Sleep problems were measured in the present study also using the SHCI. Participants rated the extent to which they had been suffering from sleep problems in the past 30 days on a Likert scale ranging from (0) not at all to (3) seriously.
Neuroticism
The neuroticism subscale of the big five inventory (BFI; [57] ) was used to collect information on this personality trait. For each of the eight items of the subscale, participants were asked to respond on a Likert scale ranging from (1) strongly disagree to (5) strongly agree. The neuroticism subscale had acceptable internal consistency in the present study (Cronbach's α =0.76).
Demographics and life-style questions
Participants were asked to report their age in years, gender, ethnic identification, level of education and current employment status.
Statistical approach
Bootstrapping estimates of indirect effects were used in the analyses of the present study. [58] The moderated mediation model [Figure 1] was tested using the process SPSS macro for 5000 samples provided by Preacher and Hayes. [59] The macro allows the estimation of conditional indirect effects. The output of the macro provides unstandardized regression coefficients and associated P values, as well as bias corrected and accelerated confidence intervals (BCa CI) for indirect effects. A significant effect occurs when the CI does not cross zero.
Subsequent models in the analyses were tested using the indirect SPSS macro. [58] In addition to calculating coefficients for the unique pathways among the variables, the macros calculate pairwise contrasts of the indirect effects and therefore the magnitudes of the indirect effect of the mediators can be compared. There is a significant difference in magnitude of the indirect effect of the mediators when the CI of the pairwise contrast does not cross zero. [59]
Controlling for confounding variables
Neuroticism was a particularly important covariate to include in the models as negative affectivity is closely related to noise sensitivity. [19],[27] In addition, for any models testing the influence of personality and environmental variables on health, it is theoretically advantageous to control for the influence of age and socio-economic status. [60] In the present study, education was used as a proxy for socio-economic status, as the collection of such information is often preferred over asking participants to indicate income or value of their assets. [61] Age was also entered as a covariate in each of the models.
Results
Descriptive statistics
Descriptive socio-demographic statistics are presented in [Table 1]. A total of 1,102 participants, ranging in age from 18 to 94 years (M = 51.39, SD = 16.42), took part in the study. The majority of the sample was New Zealand European (64.9%) and had at least some university or post-secondary education (71.2%). Descriptive statistics for the health and psychological variables in the study are presented in [Table 2].{Table 1}{Table 2}
Noise sensitivity and diminished health model
The overall model [Figure 1] was significant, F (6, 1008) =91.76, P < 0.001, R 2 =0.353 indicating that the model predictors (including possible moderators and mediators) explained 35.3% of variance in health complaints. However, the moderated regression analyses (i.e., the influence of gender/noise exposure) revealed that the noise sensitivity × noise exposure and noise sensitivity × gender interactions were not significant in predicting the model mediators [[Table 3]; interaction P > 0.05]. Due to the non-significant interaction between noise sensitivity and the moderators, the conditional indirect effects were not examined. The indirect effect of noise sensitivity on health complaints through perceived stress and sleep problems did not depend on noise exposure or gender.{Table 3}
Due to the lack of significant moderator effects in the overall moderated mediation model, a mediation model of noise sensitivity and health complaints was then tested to investigate the influence of the mediators in the model [Figure 2]. Results indicated a significant model, F (6, 1014) =93.32, P < 0.001, with predictors and mediators explaining 35.2% of the variance in health complaints. The total effect (c path) of noise sensitivity on health complaints was significant, c = 0.326, P < 0.001. The direct effect (c′ path; controlling for the influence of mediators in the model) of noise sensitivity on health complaints was not significant, c′ =0.171, P = 0.053 (after mediators were tested in the model), indicating significant influence of the indirect effects of perceived stress and sleep problems in the model. In terms of the influence of the covariates on health complaints, neuroticism was positively predictive of health complaints, B = 1.316, P < 0.001. However neither age, B = −0.008, P = 0.580, nor education, B = −0.177, P = 0.513, was significant in the model.{Figure 2}
The total indirect effect was significant, abtotal = 0.155, 95% BCa CI: (0.074, 0.249). The indirect effects of noise sensitivity on health complaints via perceived stress, a 1 b 1 =0.088, 95% BCa CI: (0.024, 0.137) and sleep problems, a 2 b 2 =0.077, 95% BCa CI: (0.023, 0.143), were significant. Pairwise contrasts indicated there was no difference in strength between the indirect effect through perceived stress (a 1 b 1) compared to that via sleep problems (a 2 b 2), B = −0.0004, 95% BCa CI: (−0.075, 0.074).
Noise sensitivity and anxiety complaints
The noise sensitivity and anxiety complaints model is presented in [Figure 3]. The results of the mediation model of noise sensitivity and anxiety complaints indicated a significant model, F (6, 1013) =110.30, P < 0.001, with the model variables explaining 39.0% of the variance in anxiety complaints. However, notably neither the total effect (c = 0.012, P = 0.173) nor the direct effect (c′ = −0.001, P = 0.902) was significant, thereby suggesting that the significance of the model cannot be attributed to the mediators. Rather, the covariates accounted for the variance in anxiety complaints. Specifically, neuroticism was significant in predicting anxiety complaints, B = 0.259, P < 0.001, with elevated neuroticism being predictive of anxiety complaints. Age was also positively predictive of anxiety complaints, B = 0.004, P = 0.009, with increases in age being associated with increased anxiety. Education was not significant in the overall model, B = 0.042, P = 0.093.{Figure 3}
The overall indirect effect was significant, abtotal = 0.013, 95% BCa CI: (0.006, 0.021), a result suggesting a significant difference between the total (c path) and direct effects (c′). This difference was accounted for by the indirect effect via both perceived stress, a1b1 = 0.007, 95% BCa CI: (0.003, 0.013) and the indirect effect via sleep problems, a2b2 = 0.006, 95% BCa CI: (0.001, 0.011). Pairwise contrasts revealed no significant difference in strength between the indirect effect via perceived stress compared with sleep problems, B = 0.001, 95% BCa CI: (−0.005, 0.008).
Noise sensitivity and depression complaints
The mediation model of noise sensitivity and depression complaints was significant, F (6, 1014) =112.53, P < 0.001, with model variables explaining 39.6% of variance in depression complaints [Figure 4]. Neither the total effect of noise sensitivity on depression complaints (c = 0.003, P = 0.683) nor the direct effect (c′ =−0.008, P = 0.284) was significant. However again, neuroticism, included in the model as a covariate, was significant in predicting depression complaints, B = 0.270, P < 0.001. The remaining covariates, age (B = −0.001, P = 0.464) and education (B = 0.007, P = 0.781), were not significant in predicting depression complaints.{Figure 4}
The overall indirect effect of noise sensitivity on depression complaints through the mediators was significant, abtotal = 0.012, 95% BCa CI: (0.006, 0.019) indicating a significant difference between the c and c′ paths. The indirect effects via perceived stress, a1b1 = 0.007, 95% BCa CI: (0.003, 0.013) and sleep problems were significant, a2 b2 = 0.004, 95% BCa CI: (0.001, 0.009). Pairwise contrasts of the indirect effects revealed that the indirect effect through sleep problems was not stronger than that via perceived stress, B = 0.003, 95% BCa CI (−0.003, 0.009).
Discussion
The present study tested whether a number of variables act as mediators and moderators in the association between noise sensitivity and health problems. As expected, greater noise sensitivity was associated with more health complaints. Results of the study indicated that the proposed moderators, namely gender and noise exposure, did not impact the model significantly. Rather, perceived stress and sleep problems were significant mediators in the association between noise sensitivity and subjective health complaints. The results for the mental health complaints models indicated that neuroticism, included in the models as a covariate, largely accounted for the association between noise sensitivity and anxiety and depression complaints.
Notably, our results indicated that stress plays an important role in the relationship between noise sensitivity and health complaints. Although the relationships among noise sensitivity, physiological stress (i.e., activation of the stress response) and psychological stress (i.e., perceived stress levels) have been tested within laboratory settings, [29],[62] the present study provides evidence for the role of perceived stress within a large community-based study and in relation to the association between noise sensitivity and health complaints.
While research indicates a close relationship between the experience of psychological stress and the physiological stress response, [63] it must be emphasized that, in our study, information was collected on perceptions of stress, not physiological stress parameters. Therefore, while we propose that the relationship between stress and health complaints is likely due to allostatic processes (i.e., bodily wear and tear from stress); [64],[65] we cannot be certain due to the lack of biomarkers in the study. However, previous laboratory studies have demonstrated a relationship between noise sensitivity and stress reactivity, as evidenced by hyperactivation of the sympathetic-adrenal-medullary system and the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis in response to psychological or environmental stressors. [29],[66],[67] Further research on the physiological strain of chronic stress in relation to the association between noise sensitivity and health will help to elucidate the mechanisms underlying this relationship.
While it is rather than well-known that sufficient quality and quantity sleep help buffer risk for health problems, [68] the results of our study extend this finding to the relationship between noise sensitivity and health problems. [9],[45],[69] In other words, noise sensitivity is associated with sleep problems, which, in turn, can lead to poor health. Indeed, sleep disturbance has been associated with noise sensitivity in both field and laboratory studies. [36],[39] Noise sensitive individuals may experience greater sleep disturbance because of greater stress reactivity, [28],[30] which, in turn, may contribute to increased awakenings throughout the night. These awakenings could be due to nighttime noise either inside or outside the home. Additional research focused on the relationship between noise sensitivity and sleep in domestic settings, would be advantageous in further understanding the relationship.
Kishikawa et al., [9] authors of a recent study on the association among noise exposure, noise sensitivity and minor psychiatric disorder (as assessed by the general health questionnaire [GHQ]; [70] ) proposed that the likely mechanism explaining the link between noise sensitivity and mental health problems was sleep disturbance. In their study, noise sensitive individuals who were exposed to considerable road traffic noise (>55 dB) were more likely to report psychiatric complaints (e.g., anxiety and insomnia, somatic symptoms) compared to non-noise sensitive participants. Due to the relationship between noise sensitivity and mental health problems, particularly among noise-exposed participants, it is reasonable that Kishikawa et al. [9] suggested sleep disturbance as a likely mediator. However, noise exposure was not a significant moderator in the present study and therefore we cannot conclude that the relationship between noise sensitivity and health complaints is attributable to sleep disturbance due to road traffic noise.
The relationship between noise sensitivity and mental health complaints in the present study was accounted for by neuroticism, included as a covariate in the models. This finding is consistent with the previous research by Smith et al., [27] who reported that the relationship between noise sensitivity and self-reported health could be accounted by negative affectivity. Smith et al. [27] used self-reported physical illness and the GHQ (which measures anxiety, severe depression, social dysfunction and somatic complaints) as health outcomes. Specifically, the use of a largely mental health-orientated scale (GHQ) may account for the similar results between their study and the results from our mental health complaints models.
Indeed, the models tested in our study suggest that the relationship between noise sensitivity and mental health complaints is accounted for by neuroticism. These results might also be explained by the high degree of shared variance among neuroticism, mental health complaints and noise sensitivity, the common thread being anxiety. However, to contrast the research findings of Smith et al., [27] in our study, neuroticism did not have the same influence over the relationship between noise sensitivity and subjective health complaints (i.e., sleep problems and perceived stress mediated the relationship even after controlling for neuroticism).
Limitations
Although the present study examined factors involved in the relationship between noise sensitivity and diminished heath relationship using a large (n > 1000) community-based study, there are some limitations with the use of this research design. First, as with all cross-sectional studies, the assumption of causality within the study models should be considered with caution. While the models were structured to suggest that noise sensitivity precedes the development of stress, sleep problems and health complaints, it is not possible to confidently confirm the direction of the associations in this study. Further research on the topic, longitudinal designs in particular, would be beneficial in discerning the nature of the relationship between noise sensitivity and health problems.
Second, we used road traffic volume as a proxy for noise exposure in the present study, which is not the ideal method for assessing environmental noise exposure. The methods for collecting information on environmental noise exposure range from relatively precise (e.g., personal dosimeters; [71],[72] to the moderately accurate (e.g., Nordic prediction method; [73] ) to the relatively crude (e.g., participant self-reports of the type of street on which they live; [74] ).
In this study, we used road traffic volume as a proxy for noise exposure, which could be classified as a relatively crude measure of environmental noise exposure. It is possible that this could be a factor contributing to the lack of interaction between noise sensitivity and noise exposure in our model. We must emphasize, however, that noise exposure remains an important variable to consider in related models because of its theoretical relevance in the relationship between noise sensitivity and health. Further research on the relationship between noise sensitivity and health should consider using more precise methods of environmental noise exposure, such as personal dosimeters, in order to fully understand the strain of various forms of noise exposure on noise sensitive individuals.
Another limitation of our study is the use of a subjective health complaints scale as a health outcome. These types of scales require the researchers to rely on the participant's own perception of their health and well-being, which can be viewed as less accurate than more objective health outcomes such as physician-diagnosed illnesses and laboratory-tested conditions (e.g., hypertension, cancer, autoimmune disease). However, the use of health complaints scales is a common and reliable method to assess health problems. [75],[76],[77] Further, they are strong predictors of visits to a medical physician and taking sick leave from work, [78] thus highlighting the relevance to public health research. Nonetheless, future research on noise sensitivity and health using more objective outcomes will help to appropriately expand this area of research.
Finally, the low response rate is a limitation of the study. Galea and Tracy [79] discussed the issue of declining participation rates in recent years. In particular, they suggested that people are now less likely to volunteer for studies due to the steep increase in survey-based research conducted by universities and government bodies. In other words, the oversampling of the general population has led to declining response rates. Auckland is the largest city in New Zealand and home to three universities, so it is possible that our low response rate could be reflective of oversampling and in turn, declining interest to participate in research among residents of Auckland, New Zealand.
There are some strategies that can be employed to help to increase participation in survey-based studies, such as offering financial incentives or extensive follow-up with participants. [79] Participants completed the questionnaire anonymously and therefore extensive follow-up was unfortunately not feasible. Despite a low response rate, our large sample had a good range of data, which allowed for the testing of our statistical models. Overall, these limitations should be considered in interpreting the results of the study.
Conclusion
While previous researchers have reported an association between noise sensitivity and diminished health, [10],[11],[12],[13],[45] there has been limited attention as to the mechanisms underlying the relationship. The present study fills an important gap in the literature through the assessment of mediators and moderators of the association between noise sensitivity and diminished health using a large community-based study. Perceived stress and sleep problems significantly mediated the association between noise sensitivity and subjective health complaints, even after controlling for the influence of neuroticism. Specifically, it is possible that noise sensitive individuals are more likely to experience psychological stress, which, in turn, places them at risk for developing health problems. Further research to elucidate physiological agents that may be involved will help to provide more understanding. In addition, research using more precise methods of noise exposure (e.g., personal dosimeters) will help to further understand the impact of noise on the relationship among noise sensitivity, stress and health problems. Overall, our study provides useful insight into the psychological factors involved in the relationship between noise sensitivity and diminished health and identifies areas for further study in the field.
Acknowledgements
We would like to acknowledge Dr. David McBride from the University of Otago Department of Preventive and Social Medicine and Dr. Kim Dirks from the University of Auckland School of Population Health for their financial support in this research project.
References
1 | Lercher P. Environmental noise and health: An integrated research perspective. Environ Int 1996;22:117-29. |
2 | Stansfeld SA, Sharp DS, Gallacher J, Babisch W. Road traffic noise, noise sensitivity and psychological disorder. Psychol Med 1993;23:977-85. |
3 | Ohrström E, Björkman M, Rylander R. Noise annoyance with regard to neurophysiological sensitivity, subjective noise sensitivity and personality variables. Psychol Med 1988;18:605-13. |
4 | Weinstein ND. Individual differences in reactions to noise: A longitudinal study in a college dormitory. J Appl Psychol 1978;63:458-66. |
5 | Welch D, Shepherd D, Dirks KN, McBride D, Marsh S. Road traffic noise and health-related quality of life: A cross-sectional study. Noise Health 2013;15:224-30. |
6 | Guski R. Personal and social variables as co-determinants of noise annoyance. Noise Health 1999;1:45-56. |
7 | Job RF. Community response to noise: A review of factors influencing the relationship between noise exposure and reaction. J Acoust Soc Am 1988;83:991-1001. |
8 | Paunoviæ K, Jakovljeviæ B, Belojeviæ G. Predictors of noise annoyance in noisy and quiet urban streets. Sci Total Environ 2009;407:3707-11. |
9 | Kishikawa H, Matsui T, Uchiyama I, Miyakawa M, Hiramatsu K, Stansfeld SA. Noise sensitivity and subjective health: Questionnaire study conducted along trunk roads in Kusatsu, Japan. Noise Health 2009;11:111-7. |
10 | Fyhri A, Klaeboe R. Road traffic noise, sensitivity, annoyance and self-reported health: A structural equation model exercise. Environ Int 2009;35:91-7. |
11 | Heinonen-Guzejev M, Vuorinen HS, Mussalo-Rauhamaa H, Heikkilä K, Koskenvuo M, Kaprio J. Somatic and psychological characteristics of noise-sensitive adults in Finland. Arch Environ Health 2004;59:410-7. |
12 | Heinonen-Guzejev M, Vuorinen HS, Mussalo-Rauhamaa H, Heikkilä K, Koskenvuo M, Kaprio J. The association of noise sensitivity with coronary heart and cardiovascular mortality among Finnish adults. Sci Total Environ 2007;372:406-12. |
13 | Nivison ME, Endresen IM. An analysis of relationships among environmental noise, annoyance and sensitivity to noise, and the consequences for health and sleep. J Behav Med 1993;16:257-76. |
14 | Heinonen-Guzejev M, Koskenvuo M, Silventoinen K, Mussalo-Rauhamaa H, Vuorinen HS, Heikkilä K, et al. Noise sensitivity and disability retirement: A longitudinal twin study. J Occup Environ Med 2013;55:365-70. |
15 | Job RF. Noise sensitivity as a factor influencing human reaction to noise. Noise Health 1999;1:57-68. |
16 | Al-Mutairi NZ, Al-Attar MA, Al-Rukaibi FS. Traffic-generated noise pollution: Exposure of road users and populations in Metropolitan Kuwait. Environ Monit Assess 2011;183:65-75. |
17 | Fyhri A, Aasvang GM. Noise, sleep and poor health: Modeling the relationship between road traffic noise and cardiovascular problems. Sci Total Environ 2010;408:4935-42. |
18 | Ellermeier W, Eigenstetter M, Zimmer K. Psychoacoustic correlates of individual noise sensitivity. J Acoust Soc Am 2001;109:1464-73. |
19 | Stansfeld SA. Noise, noise sensitivity and psychiatric disorder: Epidemiological and psychophysiological studies. Psychol Med 1992;Suppl 22:1-44. |
20 | Moreira NM, Bryan ME . Noise annoyance susceptibility. J Sound Vib 1972;21:449-62. |
21 | Stansfeld SA, Clark CR, Turpin G, Jenkins LM, Tarnopolsky A. Sensitivity to noise in a community sample: II. Measurement of psychophysiological indices. Psychol Med 1985;15:255-63. |
22 | Thomas JR, Jones DM. Individual differences in noise annoyance and the uncomfortable loudness level. J Sound Vib 1982;82:289-304. |
23 | Zimmer K, Ellermeier W. Psychometric properties of four measures of noise sensitivity: A comparison. J Environ Psychol 1999;19:295-302. |
24 | Belojevic G, Jakovljevic B. Factors influencing subjective noise sensitivity in an urban population. Noise Health 2001;4:17-24. |
25 | Dornic S, Ekehammar B. Extraversion, neuroticism, and noise sensitivity. Pers Individ Dif 1990;11:989-92. |
26 | McCrae RR, Costa PT Jr. Validation of the five-factor model of personality across instruments and observers. J Pers Soc Psychol 1987;52:81-90. |
27 | Smith A, Nutt D, Wilson S, Rich N, Hayward S, Heatherley S. Noise and Insomnia: A Study of Community Noise Exposure, Sleep Disturbance, Noise Sensitivity and Subjective Reports of Health. Cardiff, UK: Report to the UK Department of Health and Department of Environment, Transport and the Regions; 2002. |
28 | Griefahn B, Di Nisi J. Mood and cardiovascular functions during noise, related to sensitivity, type of noise and sound pressure level. J Sound Vib 1992;155:111-23. |
29 | Ljungberg JK, Neely G. Stress, subjective experience and cognitive performance during exposure to noise and vibration. J Environ Psychol 2007;27:44-54. |
30 | Waye KP, Bengtsson J, Rylander R, Hucklebridge F, Evans P, Clow A. Low frequency noise enhances cortisol among noise sensitive subjects during work performance. Life Sci 2002;70:745-58. |
31 | Barnes PJ, Adcock IM. Glucocorticoid resistance in inflammatory diseases. Lancet 2009;373:1905-17. |
32 | Cohen S, Janicki-Deverts D, Miller GE. Psychological stress and disease. JAMA 2007;298:1685-7. |
33 | Hardoy MC, Carta MG, Marci AR, Carbone F, Cadeddu M, Kovess V, et al. Exposure to aircraft noise and risk of psychiatric disorders: The Elmas survey: Aircraft noise and psychiatric disorders. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol 2005;40:24-6. |
34 | McEwen BS, Stellar E. Stress and the individual. Mechanisms leading to disease. Arch Intern Med 1993;153:2093-101. |
35 | Meerlo P, Sgoifo A, Suchecki D. Restricted and disrupted sleep: Effects on autonomic function, neuroendocrine stress systems and stress responsivity. Sleep Med Rev 2008;12:197-210. |
36 | Marks A, Griefahn B. Associations between noise sensitivity and sleep, subjectively evaluated sleep quality, annoyance, and performance after exposure to nocturnal traffic noise. Noise Health 2007;9:1-7. |
37 | Öhrström E, Björkman M . Effects of noise-disturbed sleep: A laboratory study on habituation and subjective noise sensitivity. J Sound Vib 1988;122:277-90. |
38 | Öhrström E. Effects of low levels of road traffic noise during the night: A laboratory study on number of events, maximum noise levels and noise sensitivity. J Sound Vib 1995;179:603-15. |
39 | Aasvang GM, Moum T, Engdahl B. Self-reported sleep disturbances due to railway noise: Exposure-response relationships for nighttime equivalent and maximum noise levels. J Acoust Soc Am 2008;124:257-68. |
40 | Belojeviæ G, Jakovljeviæ B, Aleksiæ O. Subjective reactions to traffic noise with regard to some personality traits. Environ Int 1997;23:221-6. |
41 | Öhrström E, Rylander R, Björkman M. Effects of night time road traffic noise: An overview of laboratory and field studies on noise dose and subjective noise sensitivity. J Sound Vib 1988;127:441-8. |
42 | Shepherd D, Welch D, Dirks KN, Mathews R. Exploring the relationship between noise sensitivity, annoyance and health-related quality of life in a sample of adults exposed to environmental noise. Int J Environ Res Public Health 2010;7:3579-94. |
43 | Öhrström E, Björkman M, Rylander R. Effects of noise during sleep with reference to noise sensitivity and habituation. Environ Int 1990;16:477-82. |
44 | Babisch W . Noise sensitivity in cardiovascular noise studies. Lisbon, Portugal: Inter-Noise; 2010. |
45 | Schreckenberg D, Griefahn B, Meis M. The associations between noise sensitivity, reported physical and mental health, perceived environmental quality, and noise annoyance. Noise Health 2010;12:7-16. |
46 | McEwen BS. Central effects of stress hormones in health and disease: Understanding the protective and damaging effects of stress and stress mediators. Eur J Pharmacol 2008;583:174-85. |
47 | Marshall NS, Stranges S. Sleep duration: Risk factor or risk marker for ill-health? In: Cappuccio FP, Miller MA, Lockley SW, editors. Sleep, Health, and Society: From Aetiology to Public Health. New York, NY: Oxford University Press; 2010. |
48 | McEwen BS. Protective and damaging effects of stress mediators. N Engl J Med 1998;338:171-9. |
49 | Bültmann U, Nielsen MB, Madsen IE, Burr H, Rugulies R. Sleep disturbances and fatigue: Independent predictors of sickness absence? A prospective study among 6538 employees. Eur J Public Health 2013;23:123-8. |
50 | Matthews KA, Everson-Rose SA, Kravitz HM, Lee L, Janssen I, Sutton-Tyrrell K. Do reports of sleep disturbance relate to coronary and aortic calcification in healthy middle-aged women? Study of women's health across the nation. Sleep Med 2013;14:282-7. |
51 | Ferrie JE, Kumari M, Salo P, Singh-Manoux A, Kivimäki M. Sleep epidemiology: A rapidly growing field. Int J Epidemiol 2011;40:1431-7. |
52 | Amann E, Lercher P, Weichbold V, Eisenmann A. The effects of measuring noise sensitivity with a single or several rating scale on the population prevalence and distribution of this personality trait. Istanbul, Turkey: Inter-Noise; 2007. |
53 | Eriksen HR, Ihlebaek C, Ursin H. A scoring system for subjective health complaints (SHC). Scand J Public Health 1999;27:63-72. |
54 | Ursin H, Endresen IM, Ursin G. Psychological factors and self-reports of muscle pain. Eur J Appl Physiol Occup Physiol 1988;57:282-90. |
55 | Ihlebaek C, Eriksen HR, Ursin H. Prevalence of subjective health complaints (SHC) in Norway. Scand J Public Health 2002;30:20-9. |
56 | Cohen S, Williamson GM. Perceived stress in a probability sample of the United States. In: Spacaman S, Oskamp S, editors. The Social Psychology of Health. Newbury Park, CA: Sage; 1988. p. 31-67. |
57 | John OP, Donahue EM, Kentle RL. The Big Five Inventory: Versions 4a and 54. Berkeley, CA: University of California, Berkeley, Institute of Personality and Social Research; 1991. |
58 | Preacher KJ, Rucker DH, Hayes AF. Addressing moderated mediation hypotheses: Theory, methods, and prescriptions. Multivariate Behav Res 2007;42:185-227. |
59 | Preacher KJ, Hayes AF. Asymptotic and resampling strategies for assessing and comparing indirect effects in multiple mediator models. Behav Res Methods 2008;40:879-91. |
60 | Pickett KE, Pearl M. Multilevel analyses of neighbourhood socioeconomic context and health outcomes: A critical review. J Epidemiol Community Health 2001;55:111-22. |
61 | Grzywacz JG, Almeida DM, Neupert SD, Ettner SL. Socioeconomic status and health: A micro-level analysis of exposure and vulnerability to daily stressors. J Health Soc Behav 2004;45:1-16. |
62 | Waye KP, Clow A, Edwards S, Hucklebridge F, Rylander R. Effects of nighttime low frequency noise on the cortisol response to awakening and subjective sleep quality. Life Sci 2003;72:863-75. |
63 | Kemeny ME. The psychobiology of stress. Curr Dir Psychol Sci 2003;12:124-9. |
64 | McEwen BS. Stress, adaptation, and disease. Allostasis and allostatic load. Ann N Y Acad Sci 1998;840:33-44. |
65 | Cohen S, Janicki-Deverts D. Who's stressed? Distributions of psychological stress in the United States in probability samples from 1983, 2006, and 2009. J Appl Soc Psychol 2012;42:1320-34. |
66 | Di Nisi J, Muzet A, Weber LD. Cardiovascular responses to noise: Effects of self-estimated sensitivity to noise, sex, and time of the day. J Sound Vib 1987;114:271-9. |
67 | Ising H, Dienel D, Günther T, Markert B. Health effects of traffic noise. Int Arch Occup Environ Health 1980;47:179-90. |
68 | Lockley SW . Principles of sleep-wake regulation. In: Cappuccio FP, Miller MA, Lockley SW, editors. Sleep, Health, and Society: From Aetiology to Public Health. New York: Oxford University Press; 2010. |
69 | Heinonen-Guzejev M . Noise Sensitivity-Medical, Psychological and Genetic Aspects. Helsinki, Finland. University of Helsinki; 2009. |
70 | Goldberg DP. The Detection of Psychiatric Illness by Questionnaire. London, UK: Oxford University Press; 1972. |
71 | Chang TY, Lai YA, Hsieh HH, Lai JS, Liu CS. Effects of environmental noise exposure on ambulatory blood pressure in young adults. Environ Res 2009;109:900-5. |
72 | Weinmann T, Ehrenstein V, von Kries R, Nowak D, Radon K. Subjective and objective personal noise exposure and hypertension: An epidemiologic approach. Int Arch Occup Environ Health 2012;85:363-71. |
73 | Bendtsen H . The Nordic prediction method for road traffic noise. Sci Total Environ 1999;235:331-8. |
74 | Babisch W, Schulz C, Seiwert M, Conrad A. Noise annoyance as reported by 8- to 14-year-old children. Environ Behav 2010;44:68-86. |
75 | Witvliet MI, Kunst AE, Stronks K, Arah OA. Variations between world regions in individual health: A multilevel analysis of the role of socio-economic factors. Eur J Public Health 2012;22:284-9. |
76 | Lovell B, Moss M, Wetherell MA. Perceived stress, common health complaints and diurnal patterns of cortisol secretion in young, otherwise healthy individuals. Horm Behav 2011;60:301-5. |
77 | van den Berg AE, Maas J, Verheij RA, Groenewegen PP. Green space as a buffer between stressful life events and health. Soc Sci Med 2010;70:1203-10. |
78 | Eriksen HR, Ursin H. Subjective health complaints, sensitization, and sustained cognitive activation (stress). J Psychosom Res 2004;56:445-8. |
79 | Galea S, Tracy M. Participation rates in epidemiologic studies. Ann Epidemiol 2007;17:643-53. |
|